Volume 20, Issue 22, Pages (November 2010)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 17, Issue 8, Pages (April 2007)
Advertisements

Wei-Hsiang Lin, Edo Kussell  Current Biology 
Cursive Writing with Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Maternal Regulation of Infant Brain State
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Volume 64, Issue 3, Pages (November 2009)
Volume 26, Issue 14, Pages (July 2016)
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation
Volume 26, Issue 4, Pages (February 2016)
The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation
Volume 17, Issue 21, Pages (November 2007)
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages e3 (October 2017)
Volume 17, Issue 13, Pages (July 2007)
Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages e3 (September 2017)
Volume 20, Issue 23, Pages (December 2010)
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Waking-like Brain Function in Embryos
Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System
Human Cortical Neurons in the Anterior Temporal Lobe Reinstate Spiking Activity during Verbal Memory Retrieval  Anthony I. Jang, John H. Wittig, Sara.
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
Disruption of Perceptual Learning by a Brief Practice Break
Effects of Locomotion Extend throughout the Mouse Early Visual System
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages (January 2007)
Anubhuti Goel, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Acetylcholine Mediates Behavioral and Neural Post-Error Control
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Volume 74, Issue 4, Pages (May 2012)
Ryota Kanai, Tom Feilden, Colin Firth, Geraint Rees  Current Biology 
Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Consequences of the Oculomotor Cycle for the Dynamics of Perception
Single-Molecule Tracking of Collagenase on Native Type I Collagen Fibrils Reveals Degradation Mechanism  Susanta K. Sarkar, Barry Marmer, Gregory Goldberg,
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Martijn Barendregt, Ben M. Harvey, Bas Rokers, Serge O. Dumoulin 
Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
Traces of Experience in the Lateral Entorhinal Cortex
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Ben Vermaercke, Hans P. Op de Beeck  Current Biology 
Akinori Mitani, Mingyuan Dong, Takaki Komiyama  Current Biology 
Volume 26, Issue 9, Pages (May 2016)
Volume 21, Issue 11, Pages (June 2011)
Volume 17, Issue 15, Pages (August 2007)
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Social Attention and the Brain
The Perception and Misperception of Specular Surface Reflectance
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Evelina Fedorenko, John Duncan, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Anubhuti Goel, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Lior Reich, Marcin Szwed, Laurent Cohen, Amir Amedi  Current Biology 
Sharing Social Touch in the Primary Somatosensory Cortex
Brain Structure Links Loneliness to Social Perception
Simon Hanslmayr, Jonas Matuschek, Marie-Christin Fellner 
Jonathan Redshaw, Thomas Suddendorf  Current Biology 
Clark Fisher, Winrich A. Freiwald  Current Biology 
James N. Ingram, Ian S. Howard, J. Randall Flanagan, Daniel M. Wolpert 
Spatiotemporal Neural Pattern Similarity Supports Episodic Memory
New Insights in Human Memory Interference and Consolidation
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 20, Issue 22, Pages 2016-2020 (November 2010) Modulating Neuronal Activity Produces Specific and Long-Lasting Changes in Numerical Competence  Roi Cohen Kadosh, Sonja Soskic, Teresa Iuculano, Ryota Kanai, Vincent Walsh  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 22, Pages 2016-2020 (November 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.007 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 A Schematic Outline of the Experimental Design in a Typical Daily Session (A) TDCS was delivered for 20 min from the start of the training. In this case, anodal stimulation was applied to the right parietal lobe (red arrow), whereas cathodal stimulation was delivered to the left parietal lobe (blue arrow). (B) The training continued after the termination of the stimulation. (C and D) Once the training ended, the subjects performed the numerical Stroop task (C) and the number-to-space task (D). The time next to each image reflects the elapsed time from the beginning of the daily session until its termination in a cumulative fashion. Current Biology 2010 20, 2016-2020DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.007) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 The Congruity Effect for the Artificial Digits, the Cumulative Congruity Effect over Training, and the Congruity Effect for Everyday Digits for the Sham, RC-LA, and RA-LC Groups in the Numerical Stroop Task The data of the artificial digits for each group are averaged across the sessions that showed a significant congruity effect (three sessions for the RA-LC group, two sessions for the sham group, and five sessions for the RC-LA group; note that the latter group showed an abnormal congruity effect that was not changed as a function of learning), and the raw data, which includes RTs in each session for each group, are presented in Table S1. (A) Whereas the RA-LC group and the sham group showed a typical congruity effect, the RC-LA group showed an abnormal effect that mirrored the performance of children at the age of 6 years and might reflect perceptual rather than semantic interference [22]. (B) The cumulative congruity effect demonstrates the emergence of a consistent automatic numerical processing already from the fourth day for the RA-LC group (p = 0.005, Table S1), whereas it occurred only later for the sham group (p = 0.049, Table S1) and did not appear for the RC-LA group. (C) All groups showed a consistent and typical congruity effect for everyday digits (p = 0.00009; group x congruity interaction, p = 0.46), as reflected by slower RTs for the incongruent condition versus the congruent condition. Data are mean ± standard error (SE) of the mean. Note the different scaling in each panel. For a description of the task, see Figure S2. Current Biology 2010 20, 2016-2020DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.007) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Average Location of Artificial Digits on the Horizontal Segment, Shown Separately for Artificial Digits in the Left Column, Everyday Digits in the Right Column, and Type of Stimulation β represents the selection of the best weight, whether it was logarithmic (βlog) or linear (βlin), in stepwise regression analysis with linear and logarithmic predictors. Data are mean ± SE of the mean. The first row reflects the performance of the RA-LC group (red circles), the middle row reflects the performance of the sham group (black circles), and the bottom row presents the performance of the RC-LA group (blue circles). Whereas the performance with artificial digits was affected by the type of brain stimulation and showed a linear fit only for the RA-LC group, the performance with everyday digits was independent of the type of brain stimulation and showed a linear fit for all the groups. For a description of the task, see Figure S3. Current Biology 2010 20, 2016-2020DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.10.007) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions