Working Draft: A “stepwise animation” illustration of cognitive load theory, followed by speculation of how it might link to instruction of written word.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Spelling Development Students’ spellings are not just random mistakes. There is an underlying logic to students’ errors that change over time, moving from.
Advertisements

Chapter 6—Phonics Kendra McLaren Doug McLaren
Maine Reading First Course
Phonics Jillian Marshall February 5, Phonics: Cracking the Code “At one magical instant in your early childhood— that string of confused, alien.
Being a Science Learning Doctor Dr. Keith Taber. the metaphor Science teaching does not always bring about intended learning - for all sorts of reasons.
Balanced Literacy J McIntyre Belize.
Planning Value of Planning What to consider when planning a lesson Learning Performance Structure of a Lesson Plan.
Principles of High Quality Assessment
Meaningful Learning in an Information Age
Word Study for Intermediate Readers and Writers: The Syllables and Affixes Stage Chapter 7.
INDUCTIVE & DEDUCTIVE RESEARCH APPROACH
Phonics. Phonics Instruction “Phonics instruction teaches children the relationship between the letters of written language and the individual sounds.
Spelling is a tool for writing Virginia Outred and Jane Denny (CSO) From David Hornsby lecture
Science Inquiry Minds-on Hands-on.
Effective Questioning in the classroom
Phonics. What is Phonics? Phonics is a strategy for teaching children how to read. Phonics is a strategy for teaching children how to read. Teaching children.
ACOS 2010 Standards of Mathematical Practice
Effects of instruction in morphology on reading Peter N. Bowers, Queen’s University John R. Kirby, Queen’s University and S. Hélène Deacon, Dalhousie University.
Supporting your child with reading.
Stages of Developmental Word Study By: Bethany Linkous Fall 2006.
The Cognitive Load Theory
Guided Reading Guided reading enables students to practice strategies with the teacher’s support, and leads to independent silent reading.
Language and Literacy Unit 4 - Getting Ready for the Unit
Phonemic Awareness and Phonics Instruction Team 7 Special Services Teachers Alabama State Department of Education.
Writing Analytically.
1546 J. HEYWOOD Prov. II. iv. (1867) 51 . —Oxford English Dictionary
Index Peter Bowers Developing literacy and critical thinking through scientific inquiry about how English spelling works. Structured Word Inquiry.
Working Draft: A “stepwise animation” illustration of cognitive load theory, followed by speculation of how it might link to instruction of written word.
Fourth Grade Reading Night Teaching the Five Components of Reading.
Help talk play think say do go feel ing -er-es -s-ing suffixes -ful-ed What words can we build from these bases and suffixes?
Developmental Word Knowledge
Vocabulary: Figuring Out What Words Mean Essential Question: How can we help students assimilate new words into their vocabulary beyond the classroom?
Words Their Way and Word Study in First Grade
Phase 6 Throughout Year 2. Vocabulary We use the correct terminology with the children right from reception. It may sound complicated but it actually.
End of Key Stage 1 Statutory Assessments Teacher Assessment End of KS1 assessment will be based on judgements made by teachers throughout the key.
LITERACY-BASED DISTRICT-WIDE PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Aiken County Public School District January 15, 2016 LEADERS IN LITERACY CONFERENCE.
Jeopardy Theoretical Perspectives Early LiteracyElements of Literacy Teaching Reading Potpourri Q $100 Q $200 Q $300 Q $400 Q $500 Q $100 Q $200 Q $300.
Madginford Primary School
When we know a word, we usually know three things about it: Orthographic Knowledge (spelling) Semantic Knowledge (meaning) Phonological Knowledge (sound)
1 Applying Principles To Reading Presented By Anne Davidson Michelle Diamond.
Word Study With Diverse Learners What? Why? How? 2009 IRA Regional Conference: Branson, MO Presenters: Jenifer Pastore and Brandi Clowers.
Copyright 2012 Pearson Education. Vukelich, Helping Young Children Learn Language and Literacy: Birth Through Kindergarten 3/e Chapter 1 Foundations of.
Make Sense of Spelling and Spell Well!. What Does Research Say? The spelling system of English makes sense... most of the time! (Words that are related.
FOUNDATION STAGE 2 Reading and Phonics Meeting for Parents As a parent, your involvement in supporting your child’s learning will be a vital factor in.
1 Guided Reading Elizabeth Olsen Guided Reading Lesson Component Review Questions to Deepen Comprehension.
Constructivism is a theory based on observation and scientific study about how people learn. It is a teaching philosophy based on the concept that learning.
Reading At Home Yearsley Grove Primary School
Phonics Information Spring 2017
The National Curriculum:
Greenhills Primary Literacy Workshop
Enjoy reading! 19/09/2016 Laura Foster..
Phonics EYFS and Year One Thursday 10th November 2016.
Reading Procedures: MODELLED READING
Make Sense of Spelling and Spell Well!
The Year of Core Instruction
Year 1 Phonics.
Parents, Children and Teachers Working Together
Listening listen attentively to spoken language and show understanding by joining in and responding explore the patterns and sounds of language through.
Year 2: How to help your child
Reasoning and Problem Solving Maths Café
Common Core State Standards Standards for Mathematical Practice
Analyze Student Work Sample 2 Instructional Next Steps
Cognitive Load Theory Sweller, van Merrienboer, and Paas, 1998
Analyzing Student Work Sample 2 Instructional Next Steps
The 4 systems that “clue” us into making meaning!
Using Phonemic Awareness &
Enjoy reading! 13/06/2019 Laura Foster..
Make Sense of Spelling and Spell Well!
Phonics Workshop October 2019.
Presentation transcript:

Working Draft: A “stepwise animation” illustration of cognitive load theory, followed by speculation of how it might link to instruction of written word structure Pete Bowers Nov. 12/07 This slide show is just a draft made to think through cognitive load theory and how it might relate to my research on written word structure instruction based on Real Spelling teaching tools (Ramsden, 2001). It was made light of conversations with John Kirby and these three main readings: Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 417-423. Schnotz, W & Kurschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of Cognitive Load Theory. Educational Psychology Review. 19, 496-508. Schnotz, W. & Rasch, T. (2005). Enabling, Facilitating, and Inhibiting Effects of Animations in Multimedia Learning: Why Reduction of Cognitive Load Can Have Negative Results on Learning. Educational Technology Research & Development (53) 3, 47-58

Unintegrated information in WM Unintegrated information in WM Working Memory Unintegrated information in WM Unintegrated information in WM Learner A Learner B

LTM Germane Load: Working Memory Learner A Learner B Appropriate Instructional design encourages students to engage in cognitive processing that targets the construction of well integrated mental representation of schema. (Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994; Schnotz & Kurschner, 2007)

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM Working Memory unit of WM processing Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Learner A Learner B Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Germane processing not yet begun. Germane processing begins. Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

LTM LTM Working Memory Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

Well integrated schema, is only helpful if represents how things work! Working Memory Well integrated schema, is only helpful if represents how things work! Well integrated representations in LTM LTM

For Educators to consider: Again… Appropriate Instructional design encourages students to engage in cognitive processing that targets the construction of well integrated mental representation of schema. (Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994; Schnotz & Kurschner, 2007) Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

Appropriate Instructional design encourages students to engage in cognitive processing that targets the construction of well integrated mental representation of schema. (Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994; Schnotz & Kurschner, 2007) For consideration: How well does typical instruction of the written word make use of this principle of instructional design? Might word structure instruction be a way of targeting well integrated mental representations of schemas for how the written word works to represent meaning? Prior learning has produced: Poorly integrated representations in LTM Well integrated representations in LTM LTM LTM

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping Typical classroom instruction directs cognitive processing at surface patterns of ‘letter-sound’ correspondences. These patterns seem to break down randomly on some words and are treated as irregular (e.g. does, business). “irregular” or confusing spellings do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping Some words have surface patterns that are not seen as irregular, but inconsistent letter-sound patterns make the formation of stable schemas difficult. How does a child remember which spelling (hopping or hoping) describes what rabbits do? “irregular” or confusing spellings do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping Without knowledge of the details of how word structure works, teachers can hope to help students remember spellings like these, but they can’t build a coherent understanding of the principles which govern the spelling system and apply to all complex words. “irregular” or confusing spellings do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping What are the implications for the learner when instruction is limited to surface patterns of already constructed words…… “irregular” or confusing spellings do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping …instead of focusing cognitive processing on making sense of how those surface patterns derive logically and consistently from underlying morphological structural patterns? “irregular” or confusing spellings regular written word structure do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping The surface patterns of all of these words can be clearly demonstrated as the result of extremely consistent underlying structural principles. The spellings of these words can be understood though instruction about underlying morpho-phonological structure of English spelling. “irregular” or confusing spellings regular written word structure do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping The word sum and the word matrix (following slides) are simple tools that teachers can use to point to the underlying morphological structure of any complex word. “irregular” or confusing spellings regular written word structure do + es --> does hop(p) + ing --> hopping hope/ + ing --> hoping busy/i + ness --> business

The following slides are have been adapted from presentations for teachers and parents. They show how the word sum and the word matrix draw a learner’s attention to the coherent structure of English spelling with any complex word. For example the word <does> is used as a context to teach coherent, fundamental patterns that structure all complex words. These slides are presented here to with the suggestion that this instruction seems to be an excellent match for what we know about learners’ “cognitive architecture” as described by cognitive load theory. Consider just two ways this instruction might effectively develop well integrated mental representation of written word schemas, in part by reducing extraneous load, while maximizing germane load…

1) Concrete representations of written word structure with matrices and word sums enable the use of “worked examples” of word structure. Such instruction facilitates explicit discussion, investigation and manipulation of complex patterns (germane load) while minimizing extraneous load. For example the word sum allows the learner to see each meaning unit of a complex word, how they combine (with or without suffixing changes) to create a completed spelling. This information does not need to be held in working memory while considering meaning connections and any pronunciation changes between derivations. 2) Consistent patterns are regularly reinforced with continued study. Integrated mental representations in LTM are strengthened and expanded with continued study instead of countered with frequent exceptions to previous learned rules (eg. I before e, except after c). Do the following examples fit this description?

Why might morphology be important for literacy development and instruction? It clarifies the system… Why does not duz or dose? Instruction limited to surface patterns, cannot explain the logical, consistent structure of <does>, but <duz> or <dose> are not.

Why might morphology be important for literacy development and instruction? It clarifies the system… Why does not duz or dose? do go es ne ing do+es  does do+ne  done do+ing  doing

Why might morphology be important for literacy development and instruction? Learned patterns expand to other words… do go es ne ing Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) do+es  does go+es  goes do+ne  done go+ne  gone do+ing  doing go+ing  going

The following sequence of lessons directs students’ attention to the pattern for dropping silent <e>… Matrix by Ramsden(2001) www.realspelling.com

Test hypothesis with a wider set of words… Clarify precision of the pattern. Matrix by Ramsden(2001) www.realspelling.com

Now that the consistent pattern has been established, this flow chart is used to practice this pattern systematically. Such practice aims to build coherent schemas in LTM so that recognition of this pattern can become automated. The consistent patterns for consonant doubling and y/i patterns can be taught with similar flow charts.

A sketch of “Word Structured Inquiry” Learning how to build words with meaning units helps you learn how to take written words apart into their meaning units. Word structure knowledge and strategies for orthographic investigations with the aid of references are both reinforced and expanded as teachers explicitly teach how to apply that knowledge in the study of any content area.

Students discover that lessons they learn in their explicit orthographic instruction and practice, is effective for making sense of printed words. Interesting and surprising discoveries, regularly sparked by students, are made. They identify questions to which teachers don’t know immediate answers. Students get the opportunity to observe a mature learner find answers to questions. The teacher can take advantage of this context to explicitly break down the process of working out problems.

Students discover that lessons they learn in their explicit orthographic instruction and practice, is effective for making sense of printed words. Interesting and surprising discoveries, regularly sparked by students, are made. They identify questions to which teachers don’t know immediate answers. Students get the opportunity to observe a mature learner find answers to questions. The teacher can take advantage of this context to explicitly break down the process of working out problems. The student who posed a question that was good enough that the teacher had to work to find an answer is likely to be interested in the answer. Students often work hard and independently to come up with good questions in classes learning in this way.

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” Applying word structure knowledge to new contexts With more practice breaking words in to bases and affixes, we get better at seeing morphemes inside words and problem-solving word connections like these: instruct: instructions / instructor / instructed

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” Applying word structure knowledge to new contexts And then a child notices: destruction and structure… They ask, “Don’t these connect to instruction also?”

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” Applying word structure knowledge to new contexts And then a child notices: destruction and structure… They ask, “Don’t these connect to instruction also?” Seems to be some kind of meaning connection, but the structure confuses. The pieces don’t seem to fit.

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” Applying word structure knowledge to new contexts And then a child notices: destruction and structure… They ask, “Don’t these connect to instruction also?” Seems to be some kind of meaning connection, but the structure confuses. The pieces don’t seem to fit. Strategy: Look for structure and meaning cues. - Peel off any affixes?

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” We know the prefixes <in-> and <de->… in+struction and de+struction but <struction>, doesn’t make any sense.

Example “Word Structured Inquiry” We know the prefixes <in-> and <de->… in+struction and de+struction but <struction>, doesn’t make any sense. The word <structure> could have a <-ure> suffix, and the others could use an <-ion> suffix. struct+ure; in+stuct+ion; de+struct+ion But <struct> isn’t a word either. Students have discovered something that needs teaching…

There are bases, that are never words on their own. con struct ‘build’ s ed ing ion ive or in de ob sub ure al ly Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) www.realspelling.com

in+struct + ion  instruction So, instruction, destruction and structure were connected in meaning and structure by a base that is not a word on its own… in+struct + ion  instruction re con struct ‘build’ s ed ing ion ive or in de ob sub ure al ly Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) www.realspelling.com

in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction So, instruction, destruction and structure were connected in meaning and structure by a base that is not a word on its own… in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction re con struct ‘build’ s ed ing ion ive or in de ob sub ure al ly Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) www.realspelling.com

in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction So, instruction, destruction and structure were connected in meaning and structure by a base that is not a word on its own… in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction struct+ure  structure re con struct ‘build’ s ed ing ion ive or in de ob sub ure al ly Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) www.realspelling.com

in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction So, instruction, destruction and structure were connected in meaning and structure by a base that is not a word on its own… in+struct + ion  instruction de+struct+ion  destruction struct+ure  structure Teaching this matrix introduces students to the wider word family including: construct, obstruction, substructure, instructive, instructor, structurally… re con struct ‘build’ s ed ing ion ive or in de ob sub ure al ly Word matrix (Ramsden, 2001) www.realspelling.com

Thoughts on linking Cognitive Load Theory to written word instruction: If instruction that builds cohesive, well-integrated mental schemas in the LTM facilitates efficient, effective learning, isn’t it particularly important that instruction directs students to an accurate understanding of how written word structure works? Are the teaching tools and strategies identified here “linking instruction to cognitive architecture”? Structured inquiry of underlying patterns Matrix and word sums for bringing focus to building block nature of English spelling Flow charts to practice specific patterns

Thoughts on linking Cognitive Load Theory to written word instruction: Most children learn to be successful readers and writers with the aid of instruction that is based on what would be considered the best research evidence. However, the vast majority of even the most successful students who receive this instruction will spend the rest of their literate life assuming that English spelling is irregular and doesn’t merit a great deal of attention.

Thoughts on linking Cognitive Load Theory to written word instruction: The students who fail despite what is considered best practice for classroom and remedial instruction, are not failing to remember and apply the regular morphophonolgical patterns that structure words. These children have not yet had this underlying regularity presented to them. We do not yet have research that has investigated the learning of children who learn to read and write with the support of instruction that -- from the very beginning -- constantly points to the order and structure of how English spelling works to represent meaning.

Some final “stepwise animations” of orthographic structure… Thoughts on linking Cognitive Load Theory to written word instruction: The students who fail despite what is considered best practice for classroom and remedial instruction, are not failing to remember and apply the regular morphophonolgical patterns that structure words. These children have not yet had this underlying regularity presented to them. We do not yet have research that has investigated the learning of children who learn to read and write with the support of instruction, that from the very beginning of school constantly points to the order and structure of how English spelling works to represent meaning. Some final “stepwise animations” of orthographic structure…

r Consider this presentation of two common words: Surface similarity Fundamentally different at meaning and structure level… r

re Consider this presentation of two common words: Surface similarity Fundamentally different at meaning and structure level… re

rea Consider this presentation of two common words: Surface similarity Fundamentally different at meaning and structure level… rea

reac Consider this presentation of two common words: Surface similarity Fundamentally different at meaning and structure level… reac

How do we know how to read these words? react reach ?

re + act <ea> not a digraph reach <ea> is a digraph Find quote for eye movement studies demonstrating eye is drawn to the base first

re + act <ea> not a digraph reach <ea> is a digraph Notice how the different structures offers cues for both meaning and pronunciation. react = prefix + base reach = base (<act> main meaning of the word) re + act <ea> not a digraph reach <ea> is a digraph <c> is a grapheme <ch> is a digraph

re + act <ea> not a digraph reach <ea> is a digraph Notice how the different structures offers cues for both meaning and pronunciation. react = prefix + base reach = base (<act> main meaning of the word) re + act <ea> not a digraph reach <ea> is a digraph <c> is a grapheme <ch> is a digraph Consider…

mishap misshapen mishmash mishear How do we sort out the meaning & pronunciation of these words? mishap misshapen mishmash mishear

mishap misshapen mishmash mishear Which uses <sh> and which uses <s+h>? mishap misshapen mishmash mishear

misshapen  mis+shape/+en mishmash  mish+mash mishear  mis+hear Which uses <sh> and which uses <s+h>? Structure tells us the answer mishap  mis+hap misshapen  mis+shape/+en mishmash  mish+mash mishear  mis+hear

misshapen  mis+shape/+en mishmash  mish+mash mishear  mis+hear Relevance for instruction? Can select words, not only for frequency, but also for the clarity with which they illustrate principles of how words work. mishap  mis+hap misshapen  mis+shape/+en mishmash  mish+mash mishear  mis+hear

Consider… react  re+act reach  reach mishap  mis+hap To teach ‘letter-sound correspondences’ we need to under stand the word structure too. react  re+act reach  reach mishap  mis+hap misshapen  mis+shape/+en mishmash  mish+mash mishear  mis+hear Consider…

been |bin| or |b n| teen |tin| (like <bean>) (like <bin>)

been |bin| or |b n| teen |tin| Is there a meaning cue for the varied pronunciation of the similar letter strings? been |bin| or |b n| teen |tin| (like <bean>) (like <bin>)

be+en not <ee> digraph teen <ee> digraph

be+en not <ee> digraph teen <ee> digraph Real Spelling is the only resource I’ve found that shows teachers how structure and sound work together. be+en not <ee> digraph teen <ee> digraph The <ee> digraph can only represent the ‘long e’ or |i| phoneme.

heel or heal?

heel or heal? Strategy: Think of structurally related words. One of these words has derivations that shifts from using the ‘long e’ pronunciation to the ‘short e’. That word family needs the <ea> digraph to be able to take on both jobs.

<heal> ‘base’ Practice this structure by writing word sums, whild spelling the letters out loud in their morphemic groups… Matrix by Ramsden (2001) www.realspelling.com From this link www.wordworkskingston.com http://web.mac.com/peterbowers1/iWeb/Site%207/Teacher%20Resource%20Book.html

un+heal+th+y/i+er--> unhealthier ‘base’ heal+s --> heals heal+th--> health un+heal+th+y--> unhealthy un+heal+th+y/i+er--> unhealthier Matrix by Ramsden (2001) www.realspelling.com From this link www.wordworkskingston.com http://web.mac.com/peterbowers1/iWeb/Site%207/Teacher%20Resource%20Book.html

Finally… Since this logical word structure and these instructional tools are available, is it reasonable to teach children that words like <does> or <sign> are irregular and need to be memorized? Does cognitive load theory speak to this question?