Volume 23, Issue 4, Pages (February 2013)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Sea turtles Current Biology
Advertisements

Sensory bases of navigation
Ocean currents and marine life
Volume 19, Issue 24, Pages (December 2009)
Volume 14, Issue 6, Pages R221-R224 (March 2004)
Marine Turtles Use Geomagnetic Cues during Open-Sea Homing
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Backward Masking and Unmasking Across Saccadic Eye Movements
From Compromise to Leadership in Pigeon Homing
Volume 25, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Volume 21, Issue 18, Pages (September 2011)
EEG Responses to Visual Landmarks in Flying Pigeons
With Age Comes Representational Wisdom in Social Signals
Timothy C. Roth, Aaron R. Krochmal  Current Biology 
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Tornadic Storm Avoidance Behavior in Breeding Songbirds
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e2 (September 2017)
Volume 18, Issue 3, Pages R117-R119 (February 2008)
The Role of the Magnetite-Based Receptors in the Beak in Pigeon Homing
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Walking Straight into Circles
Form and Function in Human Song
Single-Cell Analysis of Growth in Budding Yeast and Bacteria Reveals a Common Size Regulation Strategy  Ilya Soifer, Lydia Robert, Ariel Amir  Current.
Context-dependent lateralized feeding strategies in blue whales
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Longitude Perception and Bicoordinate Magnetic Maps in Sea Turtles
Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
A Magnetic Map Leads Juvenile European Eels to the Gulf Stream
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Susanne Åkesson, Jens Morin, Rachel Muheim, Ulf Ottosson 
Old-Growth Fishes Become Scarce under Fishing
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Mapping Load-Bearing in the Mammalian Spindle Reveals Local Kinetochore Fiber Anchorage that Provides Mechanical Isolation and Redundancy  Mary Williard.
Damian M. Herz, Baltazar A. Zavala, Rafal Bogacz, Peter Brown 
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Volume 21, Issue 5, Pages (March 2011)
Corticostriatal Output Gating during Selection from Working Memory
Volume 25, Issue 17, Pages R751-R752 (August 2015)
Animal Orientation Strategies for Movement in Flows
Sea turtles Current Biology
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
The Human Genetic History of the Americas: The Final Frontier
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Nikita Chernetsov, Dmitry Kishkinev, Henrik Mouritsen  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 7, Pages (April 2008)
Sriram Sankararaman, Swapan Mallick, Nick Patterson, David Reich 
Evidence that Magnetic Navigation and Geomagnetic Imprinting Shape Spatial Genetic Variation in Sea Turtles  J. Roger Brothers, Kenneth J. Lohmann  Current.
Volume 19, Issue 24, Pages (December 2009)
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic information in macaque inferior temporal cortex  Kasper Vinken, Rufin Vogels  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Motor Skills Are Strengthened through Reconsolidation
Humans Have an Expectation That Gaze Is Directed Toward Them
Repeating Spatial Activations in Human Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 27, Issue 1, Pages (January 2017)
Zuzana Burivalova, Çağan Hakkı Şekercioğlu, Lian Pin Koh 
Volume 18, Issue 3, Pages R117-R119 (February 2008)
Volume 26, Issue 9, Pages (May 2016)
Fruit-Catching Fish Tune Their Fast Starts to Compensate for Drift
Sriram Sankararaman, Swapan Mallick, Nick Patterson, David Reich 
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Richard A. Peters, Jan M. Hemmi, Jochen Zeil  Current Biology 
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 23, Issue 4, Pages 312-316 (February 2013) Evidence for Geomagnetic Imprinting as a Homing Mechanism in Pacific Salmon  Nathan F. Putman, Kenneth J. Lohmann, Emily M. Putman, Thomas P. Quinn, A. Peter Klimley, David L.G. Noakes  Current Biology  Volume 23, Issue 4, Pages 312-316 (February 2013) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.041 Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2013 23, 312-316DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.041) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Map of Study Area and Correlations of Diversion Rate with Changes in Magnetic Intensity and Sea Surface Temperature (A) Fish attempting to return to the Fraser River must travel around Vancouver Island via Queen Charlotte Strait or the Strait of Juan de Fuca. Scale bar represents 225 km. (B) Relationship between the diversion rate (the percentage of fish following the northern migratory route through the Queen Charlotte Strait) and the difference in magnetic intensity between the mouth of the Fraser River and the Queen Charlotte Strait. (C) Relationship between the diversion rate and the difference in magnetic intensity between the mouth of the Fraser River and the Strait of Juan de Fuca. (D) Relationship between the diversion rate and April sea surface temperature (SST) at Kains Island Lighthouse on northwest Vancouver Island. Trend lines are estimated by linear regression. Current Biology 2013 23, 312-316DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.041) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Maps of Magnetic Parameters that Exist at the Mouth of the Fraser River across the Northeast Pacific Ocean Insets show magnetic parameters in the immediate vicinity of Vancouver Island (scale bar represents 225 km). The white circle indicates the mouth of the Fraser River. Isolines of magnetic intensity (red) and inclination angle (blue) are based on the IGRF-11 [19]. Shaded red and blue bands assume that fish resolve intensity at ±250 nT and inclination angle at ±0.25°. Although the resolution with which salmon detect these magnetic parameters is unknown, the values shown here would average out most magnetic noise from diurnal variation, ocean currents, and anomalies from the Earth’s crust. Magnetic values are plotted assuming a 2-year ocean stage for sockeye salmon, in which fish do not compensate for secular variation (field drift) but rely on the same magnetic values that they “remember” from their initial seaward migration. Locations are shown for magnetic values that existed at the Fraser River in 1900 plotted two years later, in 1902 (A), 1951 plotted in 1953 (B), 1976 plotted in 1978 (C), and 2008 plotted in 2010 (D). Relatively few sockeye salmon used the northern route through the Queen Charlotte Strait to reach the Fraser River prior to the 1970s. However, this route has become increasingly common as the magnetic intensity isoline has drifted further into the Gulf of Alaska. Current Biology 2013 23, 312-316DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.041) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Maps Indicating Magnetic Parameters that Exist at the Mouth of the Fraser River in Relation to Idealized Salmon Distribution in the Northeast Pacific Ocean Maps depict the hypothetical interaction between salmon distribution and SST influencing the proportion of the population that encounters the magnetic intensity isoline associated with the Fraser River while in the Gulf of Alaska. Purple indicates the hypothetical distribution of salmon in the North Pacific (darker shading implies greater density). Red and blue lines indicate magnetic intensity and inclination angle isolines, respectively. The magnetic data plotted are from 1953 (A and B) and 2010 (C and D). A bicoordinate navigational strategy is likely more efficient for migrating from the open sea to the Fraser River than using a single coordinate of the magnetic field (e.g., Figure S1). However, fish that encounter the magnetic intensity isoline associated with the Fraser River could take a relatively direct route homeward by swimming along that isoline (and into the Queen Charlotte Strait). Thus, we propose that sockeye salmon use bicoordinate navigation for homing except when the fish encounter the magnetic intensity associated with the Fraser River, which signals them to swim along that isoline and thus toward home. Such a homing strategy would result in major differences in diversion rate between years and would be greatly influenced by the starting locations of fish and thus SST. In years when SST is cool, sockeye salmon are likely to be distributed widely throughout the North Pacific (A and C). Thus, in cool years, the proportion of fish that encounter the isoline of magnetic intensity associated with the Fraser River is relatively low, regardless of whether the isoline is near the coast or farther west. However, when SST is warm, sockeye are likely to be constrained to more northern latitudes (B and D), thus increasing the proportion of the population that encounters the isoline of magnetic intensity associated with the Fraser River. Based on the interaction between SST and magnetic intensity, we would expect diversion rate would be low in (A), moderate in (B), moderate in (C), and high in (D). Current Biology 2013 23, 312-316DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.041) Copyright © 2013 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions