Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Advertisements

Volume 83, Issue 1, Pages (July 2014)
Hippocampal Replay Is Not a Simple Function of Experience
David L. Barack, Steve W.C. Chang, Michael L. Platt  Neuron 
Jérôme Epsztein, Michael Brecht, Albert K. Lee  Neuron 
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Jung Hoon Sul, Hoseok Kim, Namjung Huh, Daeyeol Lee, Min Whan Jung 
Chenguang Zheng, Kevin Wood Bieri, Yi-Tse Hsiao, Laura Lee Colgin 
Volume 89, Issue 6, Pages (March 2016)
Volume 84, Issue 1, Pages (October 2014)
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
E.H. Baeg, Y.B. Kim, K. Huh, I. Mook-Jung, H.T. Kim, M.W. Jung  Neuron 
Volume 67, Issue 6, Pages (September 2010)
Seiichiro Amemiya, A. David Redish  Cell Reports 
H. Freyja Ólafsdóttir, Francis Carpenter, Caswell Barry  Neuron 
Trajectory Encoding in the Hippocampus and Entorhinal Cortex
Developmental Inhibition of Gsk3 Rescues Behavioral and Neurophysiological Deficits in a Mouse Model of Schizophrenia Predisposition  Makoto Tamura, Jun.
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
R. Ellen Ambrose, Brad E. Pfeiffer, David J. Foster  Neuron 
Single Units in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex with Anxiety-Related Firing Patterns Are Preferentially Influenced by Ventral Hippocampal Activity  Avishek.
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Decoding Cognitive Processes from Neural Ensembles
Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex
New Experiences Enhance Coordinated Neural Activity in the Hippocampus
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Jianing Yu, David Ferster  Neuron 
Annabelle C. Singer, Loren M. Frank  Neuron 
Torben Ott, Simon Nikolas Jacob, Andreas Nieder  Neuron 
Volume 90, Issue 1, Pages (April 2016)
A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its Implications
Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Dharshan Kumaran, Eleanor A. Maguire  Neuron 
Kyoko Yoshida, Nobuhito Saito, Atsushi Iriki, Masaki Isoda 
Caleb E. Strait, Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden  Neuron 
Kevin Wood Bieri, Katelyn N. Bobbitt, Laura Lee Colgin  Neuron 
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Masayuki Matsumoto, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Feng Han, Natalia Caporale, Yang Dan  Neuron 
Neural Mechanisms Underlying Human Consensus Decision-Making
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Kevin G. Guise, Matthew L. Shapiro  Neuron 
Volume 77, Issue 6, Pages (March 2013)
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
A Neural Signature of Hierarchical Reinforcement Learning
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Volume 88, Issue 4, Pages (November 2015)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Tracking Route Progression in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (September 2000)
Transient Slow Gamma Synchrony Underlies Hippocampal Memory Replay
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Volume 67, Issue 6, Pages (September 2010)
Volume 29, Issue 5, Pages e4 (March 2019)
Spatial Representation along the Proximodistal Axis of CA1
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Matthew R. Roesch, Adam R. Taylor, Geoffrey Schoenbaum  Neuron 
Volume 61, Issue 6, Pages (March 2009)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages 975-982 (December 2016) Interplay between Hippocampal Sharp-Wave-Ripple Events and Vicarious Trial and Error Behaviors in Decision Making  Andrew E. Papale, Mark C. Zielinski, Loren M. Frank, Shantanu P. Jadhav, A. David Redish  Neuron  Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages 975-982 (December 2016) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.028 Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Behavior on the Spatial Delay Discounting Task (A) Task layout. One side provides a larger reward (3×) after a delay D; the other side provides a smaller reward (1×) after 1 s. The delay is adjusted as a function of the animal’s decisions. (B1 and B2) Delay on the adjusted delay side by lap. Red indicates laps to the delayed side, increasing D by 1 s; blue indicates laps to the non-delayed side, decreasing D by 1 s. Small dots show LR and RL laps; circles show LL or RR laps. Behavior reveals three phases: investigation, titration, and exploitation. (B1) Upward titration; (B2) downward titration. (C1 and C2) Gray dots show all sampled positions in a given session, with a single lap in red. VTE can be measured quantitatively with zIdphi (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). (C1) VTE pass (zIdphi = 6.05); (C2) non-VTE pass (zIdphi = −0.13). (D) zIdphi distributed in a skewed manner but can be separated into VTE events and non-VTE events. The threshold between VTE and not was set at zIdphi = 0.5, the point where the observed distribution diverged from the expected normal distribution (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). (E) VTE decreased in the exploitation phase. Bars show interquartile range, line shows median, notch shows standard error of the median. (F) Calculated distance between paths (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Paths became more stereotyped with time. Early laps are distant from each other as well as from the later laps. Later laps are more stereotyped, marked by a lack of distance between the paths. Neuron 2016 92, 975-982DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.028) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Hippocampal Representations during VTE (A and B) While rats were at the choice point (A), we measured the mean Bayesian posterior probability over the choice point (green), the reward sites (blue), and the rest of the maze (brown) (B). The data in this figure come from all three phases. (C) While the rat was at the choice point, most of the posterior remained local. (D) The small portion of the posterior that distributed to the reward sites was significantly more distributed to the side that would subsequently be chosen. (E and F) Decoding to the reward sites (E) increased during VTE, matched by a decrease in the decoding locally at the choice point (F). (G) Decoding on the rest of the maze remained unchanged. (H) On non-VTE laps, the non-local decoding to the reward sites was preferentially distributed toward the chosen side, whereas it was more evenly distributed on VTE laps. (I) Average decoding probabilities. White boxes show the regions used to calculate the decoded posterior probability for the reward sites. Laps in which the rat went left have been flipped around the midline for display purposes. On both VTE and non-VTE passes, the majority of the decoded posterior remained at the choice point; however, the small amount of decoded posterior at the reward sites was different under VTE and non-VTE conditions. (J) The average decoded posteriors of all VTE laps Z scored against the mean and standard deviations found in the non-VTE laps. (K) Theta cycle representations encoded one side or the other but not both. For each decoded sample, we measured the proportion of the posterior assigned to each goal side (larger-later or smaller-sooner). On both VTE and non-VTE laps, when there was increased posterior to one side or the other, there was no increased posterior to the other side. This implies that the theta sequences were alternating between options serially, not simply spreading out ahead of the animal. Boxplots (C–H) show IQR (box), median (line), and standard error of the median (notch). Neuron 2016 92, 975-982DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.028) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Sharp Waves Were More Common Before and After Non-VTE Laps (A) We divided reward site experiences based on whether a VTE (zIdphi > 0.5) event occurred as the animal passed the choice point approaching the reward site experience. There was a lower rate of SWR events at the reward site after a VTE lap than after a non-VTE lap. (B) If we divided reward site experiences based on whether a VTE (zIdphi > 0.5) event occurred on the subsequent lap (following the reward site experience), there was a lower rate of SWR events before a VTE lap than a non-VTE lap. Boxplots show IQR (box), median (line), and standard error of the median (notch). Neuron 2016 92, 975-982DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.028) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 SWR Disruption Increases VTE Behavior in a Spatial Alternation Task Data re-analyzed from Jadhav et al. (2012). (A) Rats ran out from the center to a side arm (A1, outbound) and then returned to the center (A2, inbound). The following trial required visiting the alternate side arm (A3) before again returning to center (A4). (B) VTE behavior during outbound trajectories was quantified by lnIdphi (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). There were more high lnIdphi choice-point passes (VTE) in the SWR disruption animals as compared to the control group. Line and shaded area shows mean and SEM. Neuron 2016 92, 975-982DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2016.10.028) Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions