Cephalopod dynamic camouflage

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
From: Edge detection and texture classification by cuttlefish
Advertisements

Volume 20, Issue 11, Pages R461-R462 (June 2010)
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Volume 24, Issue 23, Pages R1109-R1111 (December 2014)
Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus
Imagery: Mental Pictures Disrupt Perceptual Rivalry
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Human Development: Faces in the Womb
Camouflage Current Biology
Colour Vision: Understanding #TheDress
Sexual Selection: The Importance of Long-Term Fitness Measures
Cursive Writing with Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Ecology: The Upside-Down World of Coral Reef Predators
Predatory grasshopper mice
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Information considered harmful in animal communication
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
Mimicry in plants Current Biology
Volume 27, Issue 12, Pages R599-R600 (June 2017)
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Animal Locomotion: A New Spin on Bat Flight
Insect Neurobiology: An Eye to Forward Motion
Visual Ecology: Hiding in the Dark
Infant cognition Current Biology
Ageing: It’s a Dog’s Life
Visual Perception: Lightness in a High-Dynamic-Range World
Volume 20, Issue 11, Pages R461-R462 (June 2010)
Andres Laan, Tamar Gutnick, Michael J. Kuba, Gilles Laurent 
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Face Perception: Broken into Parts
Social Learning: Public Information in Insects
Volume 18, Issue 11, Pages R453-R455 (June 2008)
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Purple Tomatoes: Longer Lasting, Less Disease, and Better for You
Evolution: Mirror, Mirror in the Pond
Colony-level cognition
Volume 18, Issue 12, Pages R527-R530 (June 2008)
Cephalopod Behaviour: Skin Flicks
Insect Vision: A Neuron that Anticipates an Object’s Path
Flies see second-order motion
Elementary motion detectors
It’s all about the constraints
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Social Dynamics: Knowledgeable Lemurs Gain Status
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Neural Coding: Sparse but On Time
FOXO transcription factors
Volume 16, Issue 15, Pages R565-R566 (August 2006)
Higher-Order Figure Discrimination in Fly and Human Vision
Visual Perception: A Novel Difference Channel in Binocular Vision
Conservation Biology: The Importance of Wilderness
Visual Ecology: Hiding in the Dark
Armillaria Current Biology
Colony-level cognition
Anemonefishes Current Biology
Dermatophytes Current Biology
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
Basal bodies Current Biology
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Cephalopod dynamic camouflage Roger Hanlon  Current Biology  Volume 17, Issue 11, Pages R400-R404 (June 2007) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034 Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Octopus vulgaris reacting to a diver (predator). The initial change from camouflaged to conspicuous takes only milliseconds due to direct neural control of the skin. Full expression of the threat display (right) is two seconds. Video frame rate is 30 frames per second. Video clip available at http://www.mbl.edu/mrc/hanlon/video.html. Current Biology 2007 17, R400-R404DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Skin papillae are evoked by visual input and can appear long and spiky, short and rounded, or not at all. Images extracted from Figure 1. Current Biology 2007 17, R400-R404DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 A visual sensorimotor assay for probing cuttlefish perception and subsequent dynamic camouflage. Row 1: visual backgrounds with different size, contrast, edge characteristics and arrangement are perceived by the cuttlefish, which quickly translates the information into a complex, highly coordinated body pattern type of uniform, mottle or disruptive (left to right in each row of photographs). Row 2: examples of how small sand particles elicit a uniform pattern in Sepia officinalis; slightly larger gravel particles of varying higher contrast elicit a mottle pattern; and large light and dark particles elicit a disruptive pattern. Row 3: simple visual stimuli — such as uniformity or small to large high-contrast checkerboards — can elicit uniform, mottle or disruptive camouflage patterns in cuttlefish. The chief difference in the latter two backgrounds is the scale of the checker. Both the visual background and the body pattern can be quantified so that correlations can be made between visual input and motor output. Row 4: enlarged images of the uniform, mottle and disruptive body patterns. Note especially the diverse shapes, orientations and contrasts in disruptive. Current Biology 2007 17, R400-R404DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 The ‘mimic octopus’ Thaumoctopus mimicus on a sand plain (left) in Indonesia. In the middle photo, the octopus takes on the shape and swimming motion of a flounder, including the fish-like undulations of body and fins and similar swimming speeds. The most common flounder in this area was Bothus mancus (right). (Photo credits: Denise Tackett for octopuses and Dave Downs for flounder.) Current Biology 2007 17, R400-R404DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.034) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions