Experiments with Two Groups

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Experimental and Ex Post Facto Designs
Advertisements

The Ways and Means of Psychology STUFF YOU SHOULD ALREADY KNOW BY NOW IF YOU PLAN TO GRADUATE.
Smith/Davis (c) 2005 Prentice Hall Chapter Thirteen Inferential Tests of Significance II: Analyzing and Interpreting Experiments with More than Two Groups.
Quasi-Experimental Designs
Validity, Sampling & Experimental Control Psych 231: Research Methods in Psychology.
Lect 10a1 Experimental Research Experimental research is conducted to demonstrate functional (cause-and-effect) relationships An experiment must demonstrate.
Sampling and Experimental Control Goals of clinical research is to make generalizations beyond the individual studied to others with similar conditions.
Chapter 9 Experimental Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
Smith/Davis (c) 2005 Prentice Hall Chapter Twelve Designing and Conducting, Experiments with More than Two Groups PowerPoint Presentation created by Dr.
Smith/Davis (c) 2005 Prentice Hall Chapter Ten Designing and Conducting, Experiments with Two Groups PowerPoint Presentation created by Dr. Susan R. Burns.
1 Psych 5500/6500 Confounding Variables Fall, 2008.
Chapter 1: Introduction to Statistics
Design Experimental Control. Experimental control allows causal inference (IV caused observed change in DV) Experiment has internal validity when it fulfills.
Chapter 8 Causal-Comparative Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian
Experimental Design All experiments have independent variables, dependent variables, and experimental units. Independent variable. An independent.
Experimental Design: One-Way Correlated Samples Design
Research Methods in Psychology Group Activity Friday August 5, 2011.
Psychological Research Strategies Module 2. Why is Research Important? Gives us a reliable, systematic way to consider our questions Helps us to draw.
AP STATISTICS Section 5.2 Designing Experiments. Objective: To be able to identify and use different experimental design techniques. Experimental Units:
Introduction section of article
1.) *Experiment* 2.) Quasi-Experiment 3.) Correlation 4.) Naturalistic Observation 5.) Case Study 6.) Survey Research.
Chapter 10 Experimental Research Gay, Mills, and Airasian 10th Edition
Simple Experiments. Causal Claim Boldest claim a scientist can make Verbs such as “associated with” and “related to” replaced with “causes, influences,
T tests comparing two means t tests comparing two means.
Chapter 8: Between Subjects Designs
The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis © 2007 Pearson Education Chapter Eleven: Designing, Conducting, Analyzing, and Interpreting Experiments.
 Allows researchers to detect cause and effect relationships  Researchers manipulate a variable and observe whether any changes occur in a second variable.
1 Chapter 11 Understanding Randomness. 2 Why Random? What is it about chance outcomes being random that makes random selection seem fair? Two things:
Some Terminology experiment vs. correlational study IV vs. DV descriptive vs. inferential statistics sample vs. population statistic vs. parameter H 0.
Methods of Presenting and Interpreting Information Class 9.
Module 2 Research Strategies
Dependent-Samples t-Test
Approaches to social research Lerum
Chapter 11: The Nuts and Bolts of one-factor experiments.
Research Methods 1 Planning Research
Chapter 5 Data Production
Stats/Methods II JEOPARDY.
Experimental Design-Chapter 8
Between-Subjects, within-subjects, and factorial Experimental Designs
Data, conclusions and generalizations
Design of Experiments.
Matched T-Test Cohen Chapter 11 For EDUC/PSY 6600.
Principles of Experiment
Chapter 13- Experiments and Observational Studies
Internal Validity – Control through
Research Methods A Method to the Madness.
2 independent Groups Graziano & Raulin (1997).
Section 5.2 Designing Experiments
Between-Subjects Experimental Designs
Designing Experiments
Experimental Design.
Experimental Design.
Daniela Stan Raicu School of CTI, DePaul University
The Nonexperimental and Quasi-Experimental Strategies
Two-Sample Between-Subjects Experiments and Independent-Samples t-Tests So far, we’ve talked about experiments in which we needed to take only one sample.
Day 1 Parameters, Statistics, and Sampling Methods
Quasi-Experimental Designs
Experimental Design: The Basic Building Blocks
What are their purposes? What kinds?
Methods of Psychological Research
Day 1 Parameters, Statistics, and Sampling Methods
Thinking critically with psychological science
Introduction to Between-Subjects Experiments
Research Methods for the Behavioral Sciences
Psychological Research Methods
Basics of Experimentation
Experiments with More Than Two Groups
Design of Experiments.
Quantitative Research Methods
Chapter Ten: Designing, Conducting, Analyzing, and Interpreting Experiments with Two Groups The Psychologist as Detective, 4e by Smith/Davis.
Presentation transcript:

Experiments with Two Groups Ryan M. Denney, Ph.D. The University of Southern Mississippi PSY 361

Two-Group Designs Experimental design The general plan for selecting participants, assigning participants to experimental conditions, controlling extraneous variables, and gathering data. Principle of parsimony (Occam’s razor) The belief that explanations of phenomena and events should remain simple until the simple explanations are no longer valid. The simplest explanation or strategy tends to be the best one

Two-Group Designs Independent variable (IV) Dependent variable (DV) A stimulus or aspect of the environment that the experimenter directly manipulated to determine its influences on behavior. Dependent variable (DV) A response or behavior that is measured. It is desired that changes in the DV be directly related to manipulation of the IV. Levels of the IV The most common manner of creating two groups with one IV is to present some amount or type of IV to one group and to withhold that IV from the second group. Thus, the presence of the IV is contrasted with the absence of the IV. These differing amounts of the IV are referred to as the levels of the IV.

Two-Group Designs Experimental group Control group In a two-group experimental design, the group of participants that receives the IV/treatment. Control group In a two-group experimental design, the group of participants that does not receive the IV. The simplest way to find out whether our IV caused a change in behavior is to compare research participants who have received the IV to others who have not If those two groups differ, and we are assured that we controlled potential extraneous variables, then we conclude that the IV caused the participants to differ.

Two-Group Designs Random Assignment: assigning research participants to groups so that each participant has an equal chance of being in any group Random assignment tends to create equal groups in the long run. As groups get larger, we can place more confidence in random assignment achieving what we want it to; that is, evenly dividing extraneous variables Different from random selection: randomly choosing participants Independent groups: random assignment of participants to groups Between-subjects comparison: comparing the performance of participants in two groups Ex: Impulse control in caffeinated vs. uncaffeinated random sample of people in Coffee Shop (compare people in both groups across age, education, SES, etc)

Random-shmandom Sometimes we are concerned that random assignment may not create equal groups Must then use “nonrandom” assignment techniques to create more equal groups Correlated assignment—used when we have only a small number of participants to choose from and we’re concerned that random assignment may not yield equal groups A method of assigning research participants to groups based on some relationship they have with each other. Used with small numbers of participants. (Ex: hetero couples in which the male has been exposed to partner violence) These small groups are then randomly assigned to treatment conditions (also known as paired or matched assignment). Ex: to receive psychoed about partner violence or not—measure relationship satisfaction

Two-Group Designs 1. Matched Pairs Research participants in a two-group design who are measured and equated on some variable before the experiment. (Ex: intro/extrovert) After we have measured this variable, we create pairs of participants that are equal on this variable. After we have created our matched pairs, we then randomly assign participants from these pairs to the different treatment conditions. (Ex: speaking to crowd)

Two-Group Designs Matched pairs example Flip coin: XX OO XX OO Measure participants along some important (possibly confounding) variable Group participants in pairs of similarity Example: gender, northerner/southerner, SUV owner/small car owner Flip coin: XX OO XX OO Experimental Group: X, O, X, O Control Group: X, O, X, O Yields equal groups

Two-Group Designs 2. Repeated measures The same participants are tested in both treatment conditions of our experiment. The matched pairs are perfectly equal because they consist of the same people or animals tested across the entire experiment. No extraneous variables (contained within participants) should be able to confound the results because any difference between the participants’ performance in the two treatment conditions should be due to the IV. Participants serve as their own controls. Impossible when you cannot completely remove the effects of the previous condition (questionnaire, memory priming) Impossible when DV cannot be measured “purely” more than once (solving a puzzle, attitudes toward …)

Two-Group Designs 3. Natural pairs Within-subjects comparison Pairs of participants are created from naturally occurring pairs (e.g. biologically or socially related). For example, twins, siblings, parents and children, husbands and wives Within-subjects comparison Refers to contrasting groups of participants who were assigned through matched pairs, natural pairs, or repeated measures. Essentially comparing scores within the same participants. (i.e., comparing each participant’s score to his/her own other score) Although this direct comparison is literally true only for repeated-measures designs, participants in matched or natural pairs are “the same” with regard to the matching variable. Ex: Math is Stressful: Student’s stress responses Preteststressful calculation test (backward from 715 by 13, “most do it in 10 minutes”)posttest Comparing subjects to themselves: controlling for: age, intelligence, time of day

Analyzing Two-Group Experiments Error variability Variability in DV scores that is due to factors other than the IV – individual differences, measurement error, and extraneous variation (also known as within- groups variability). Goal is to maximize between-groups variability and minimize error (or within-groups) variability. For a two-independent groups design, use a t test for independent samples to analyze your data. For a two-correlated-groups design, analyze your data with a t test for correlated samples (a.k.a dependent t test)

Analyzing Two-Group Experiments Assumptions for the t test for independent samples Homogeneity of variance The assumption that the variances are equal for the two (or more) groups you plan to compare statistically. Assuming the groups do not differ significantly from each other in any important way before presentation of the IV Heterogeneity of variance Occurs when the variability of the two groups was not comparable or equivalent When the groups are significantly different from each other in important ways before presentation of the IV Violates a mathematical assumption of the t statistic t tests are robust with regard to the assumption of homogeneity, meaning it can tolerate violations of its assumptions and still provide fairly accurate results.

The Point To show that two relatively equal groups differed significantly from each other for no other reason but the IV The goals is to say that there is a statistically significant likelihood that the differences observed between equal groups are so extreme and rare that they are most likely due to the IV (the “experimentally controlled difference” between the groups)