Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages R941-R945 (December 2011)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Binocular rivalry Colin W.G. Clifford Current Biology Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009) DOI: /j.cub Copyright ©
Advertisements

Sea turtles Current Biology
Emotional Memory: Selective Enhancement by Sleep
Calibrating color vision
Volume 24, Issue 23, Pages R1109-R1111 (December 2014)
The nature of Drosophila melanogaster
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Human Development: Faces in the Womb
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Human Memory: Brain-State-Dependent Effects of Stimulation
Social Behaviour: The Personalities of Groups
Neuroscience: Impaired Decision-Making in Parkinson’s Disease
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Comparative Cognition: Action Imitation Using Episodic Memory
Microbiology: Mixing Wine, Chocolate, and Coffee
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Cognitive Neurology: Stimulating Research on Neglect
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Morphogens: Precise Outputs from a Variable Gradient
Infant cognition Current Biology
Evolution: One Penis After All
Alternative Strategies: The Evolution of Switch Points
Sleep: How Many Switches Does It Take To Turn Off the Lights?
Volume 24, Issue 5, Pages R204-R206 (March 2014)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Evolution: One Penis After All
Antarctic sea ice losses drive gains in benthic carbon drawdown
Volume 19, Issue 21, Pages R971-R973 (November 2009)
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Cortical Processing: How Mice Predict the Visual Effects of Locomotion
The FEAR network Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 11, Pages R453-R455 (June 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Psychopathy Current Biology
Evolution: Mirror, Mirror in the Pond
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Sea turtles Current Biology
Sense of agency Current Biology
Time Perception: Space–Time in the Brain
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages R88-R89 (February 2017)
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
Climate Change: A Hybrid Zone Moves North
Animal Behavior: Timing in the Wild
It’s all about the constraints
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages R188-R189 (March 2015)
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Stephen G. Lomber, Blake E. Butler  Current Biology 
MicroRNA Processing: Battle of the Bulge
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 24, Issue 20, Pages R988-R993 (October 2014)
Tool Use: Crows Craft the Right Tool for the Job
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Neural basis of mathematical cognition
Volume 18, Issue 17, Pages R728-R729 (September 2008)
FOXO transcription factors
How Dopamine Enhances an Optimism Bias in Humans
Conservation Biology: The Importance of Wilderness
Dermatophytes Current Biology
Grasping Weber's law Current Biology
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
Adaptive Diversity: Hormones and Metabolism in Freshwaters
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Ctenophores Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages R941-R945 (December 2011) The optimism bias  Tali Sharot  Current Biology  Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages R941-R945 (December 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Optimism bias in healthy individuals and lack of optimism bias in depression. In two studies participants were asked to predict the likelihood of different life events that might occur to them in an upcoming month (such as receiving a gift, burning dinner, getting stuck in traffic). At the end of the month they reported back which events had occurred. (A) Healthy individuals predicted positive events to be more likely than negative or neutral events. However, in reality the likelihood of positive, negative and neutral events to occur was equal (Sharot and Dudai, cited in Sharot 2011). (B) Greater pessimism bias was associated with depressive symptoms as measured by BDI-II scores (bias is measured as the difference between predicted and actual likelihood of positive events and the reverse for negative events). Individuals scoring low on depression symptoms showed an optimism bias (dark grey), individuals showing no bias in either direction were mildly depressed (light grey), and individuals exhibiting a pessimism bias scored high on depression symptoms (white). Adapted with permission from Strunk et al. (2006). Current Biology 2011 21, R941-R945DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 How optimism is maintained in the face of reality. (A) When given desirable information about the future (such as learning that the likelihood of suffering from cancer is lower than anticipated) people updated their belief to a greater degree than when receiving undesirable information (such as learning that the likelihood of suffering from cancer is greater than anticipated). This difference was related to how well regions of the frontal lobe tracked estimation errors (that is, the differences between prior beliefs about the future and information presented). (B) Activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus was correlated with the extent of errors that resulted from positive information equally well in highly optimistic individuals and people low on optimism. (C) In the right inferior frontal gyrus activity correlated strongly with negative errors in people low on optimism, but less so in highly optimistic individuals (trait optimism is measured independently by the LOT-R scale). Adapted with permission from Sharot et al. (2011). Current Biology 2011 21, R941-R945DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions