Interpreting and Utilising Intersubject Variability in Brain Function

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
1 SSS II Lecture 1: Correlation and Regression Graduate School 2008/2009 Social Science Statistics II Gwilym Pryce
Advertisements

Integration of sensory modalities
Longitudinal Experiments Larry V. Hedges Northwestern University Prepared for the IES Summer Research Training Institute July 28, 2010.
Lecture 5 Outline – Tues., Jan. 27 Miscellanea from Lecture 4 Case Study Chapter 2.2 –Probability model for random sampling (see also chapter 1.4.1)
Ranking by Odds Ratio A Probability Model Approach let be a Boolean random variable: document d is relevant to query q otherwise Consider document d as.
Laurent Itti: CS599 – Computational Architectures in Biological Vision, USC Lecture 7: Coding and Representation 1 Computational Architectures in.
How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Luiz Pessoa Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 13, Issue 4, Pages (April 2009) DOI: /j.tics
Part 2: Model and Inference 2-1/49 Regression Models Professor William Greene Stern School of Business IOMS Department Department of Economics.
Two views of brain function Marcus E. Raichle Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 14, Issue 4, Pages (April 2010) DOI: /j.tics
Variability Introduction to Statistics Chapter 4 Jan 22, 2009 Class #4.
Statistical Analysis An Introduction to MRI Physics and Analysis Michael Jay Schillaci, PhD Monday, April 7 th, 2007.
Learning Theory Reza Shadmehr Distribution of the ML estimates of model parameters Signal dependent noise models.
Dr. Justin Bateh. Point of Estimate the value of a single sample statistics, such as the sample mean (or the average of the sample data). Confidence Interval.
James A. Roberts, Karl J. Friston, Michael Breakspear 
Analyzing and Interpreting Quantitative Data
Volume 69, Issue 3, Pages (February 2011)
Bayesian Brains without Probabilities
Cognitive neuroscience 2
The Brain Basis for Misophonia
Volume 76, Issue 5, Pages (December 2012)
Volume 55, Issue 1, Pages (July 2007)
Advances in fMRI Real-Time Neurofeedback
Integration of sensory modalities
Disruption of Large-Scale Brain Systems in Advanced Aging
Ab Initio Structure Determination from Electron Microscopic Images of Single Molecules Coexisting in Different Functional States  Dominika Elmlund, Ralph.
Models of visual word recognition
Volume 53, Issue 6, Pages (March 2007)
Unreliable Evoked Responses in Autism
Confidence as Bayesian Probability: From Neural Origins to Behavior
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Data-Driven Methods to Diversify Knowledge of Human Psychology
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Intrinsic and Task-Evoked Network Architectures of the Human Brain
Cortical Mechanisms of Smooth Eye Movements Revealed by Dynamic Covariations of Neural and Behavioral Responses  David Schoppik, Katherine I. Nagel, Stephen.
Bayesian Brains without Probabilities
Figure 3 Anatomical distribution of all implanted electrodes
Linking Memories across Time via Neuronal and Dendritic Overlaps in Model Neurons with Active Dendrites  George Kastellakis, Alcino J. Silva, Panayiota.
Deciphering Cortical Number Coding from Human Brain Activity Patterns
Medial Prefrontal and Subcortical Mechanisms Underlying the Acquisition of Motor and Cognitive Action Sequences in Humans  Etienne Koechlin, Adrian Danek,
Thomas Akam, Dimitri M. Kullmann  Neuron 
Network Neuroscience Theory of Human Intelligence
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Volume 56, Issue 1, Pages (October 2007)
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Ajay S. Pillai, Viktor K. Jirsa  Neuron 
Ajay S. Pillai, Viktor K. Jirsa  Neuron 
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Erie D. Boorman, John P. O’Doherty, Ralph Adolphs, Antonio Rangel 
Olaf Hauk, Ingrid Johnsrude, Friedemann Pulvermüller  Neuron 
Broca's Area and the Hierarchical Organization of Human Behavior
Bettina Sorger, Joel Reithler, Brigitte Dahmen, Rainer Goebel 
Advances in fMRI Real-Time Neurofeedback
Mathematical Foundations of BME Reza Shadmehr
Neural Signatures of Economic Preferences for Risk and Ambiguity
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Volume 77, Issue 3, Pages (February 2013)
Volume 76, Issue 5, Pages (December 2012)
Connectivity Fingerprints: From Areal Descriptions to Abstract Spaces
Deep Neural Networks as Scientific Models
Volume 74, Issue 1, Pages (April 2012)
Lecture 16. Classification (II): Practical Considerations
Looking for cognition in the structure within the noise
Mental Imagery: Functional Mechanisms and Clinical Applications
Common Prefrontal Regions Coactivate with Dissociable Posterior Regions during Controlled Semantic and Phonological Tasks  Brian T Gold, Randy L Buckner 
Volume 34, Issue 4, Pages (May 2002)
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Decoding Rich Spatial Information with High Temporal Resolution
A Goal Direction Signal in the Human Entorhinal/Subicular Region
Presentation transcript:

Interpreting and Utilising Intersubject Variability in Brain Function Mohamed L. Seghier, Cathy J. Price  Trends in Cognitive Sciences  Volume 22, Issue 6, Pages 517-530 (June 2018) DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003 Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Key Figure: Individual-Specific Brain Parameterization and Variability in Brain Function The brain is a dynamical system that is plastic and noisy. The changes in brain status over time are a function of its current state, the current environment or inputs, and the particular parameterisation (i.e., illustrated by the vector of parameters ‘θ’). The perpetual action of many variables generates noise that fluctuates in time [138]. For example, noise can lead to transitions between coexisting deterministic stable states or attractors [139], and, perhaps more interestingly, noise can induce new stable states that have no deterministic counterparts [138]. Each observed individual functional map is in essence the system output under a particular parameterisation for that individual. This parameterisation impacts upon cognitive states (cognitive strategies, learning styles, and expectations) and mood states (familiarity, cooperation, motivation, and stress). Because not all parameters are independent, we can reasonably assume that the number of true free parameters (i.e., degrees of freedom) is smaller. Decoding variability between subjects allows the range of some parameters ‘θ’ to be estimated. The exact modelling (e.g. generative/forward models) of this multi-input/multi-output system at the neuronal and physiological level is proving to be increasingly plausible [31,140]. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018 22, 517-530DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003) Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Segregation of Networks with Across-Subject Covariance Analyses. The figure illustrates the use of covariance analysis to segregate different networks associated with different strategies. Basically, if different personal biases for particular cognitive strategies exist, they can be distinguished from random errors by looking at similarity across brain regions in the between-subject variance. Top: different neuronal systems that can sustain the same task are dissociated using (clustering) algorithms that cluster together voxels if their associated deviations covary across subjects. Δ is the set of individual deviations from the population average. Si is an activation summary of subject i, like an effect size, after collapsing the data across time or scans, and Δi codes potential biases including the personal bias of subject i to a particular cognitive strategy. Each Δi is a whole-brain map (for subject i), and ε codes inconsistent (measurement) noise. For example, Δ (in its simplest form) can represent a set of residuals from a group (mean) analysis, or a set of eigenimages after running a principal components analysis. Bottom: some of the networks that were segregated for a semantic matching task using an unsupervised fuzzy clustering algorithm (illustrated as red-to-yellow clusters projected on anatomical axial slices). In this example, subjects were asked to indicate with a button press if visually presented words were semantically related or not. Voxels were clustered together if their associated deviations covaried across subjects. This clustering revealed many networks, including motor, visual, semantic, default mode and oculomotor networks. More details about this example can be found elsewhere [67]. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018 22, 517-530DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003) Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure I The 6D Output of a Typical Multisubject Experiment. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018 22, 517-530DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003) Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure I Averaging Images with Variable Features. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018 22, 517-530DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003) Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure II Cumulative Sums of Integers via Three Possible Strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2018 22, 517-530DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2018.03.003) Copyright © 2018 The Authors Terms and Conditions