Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages (November 2013)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Building Better Models of Visual Cortical Receptive Fields
Advertisements

Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Acute Stress Impairs Self-Control in Goal-Directed Choice by Altering Multiple Functional Connections within the Brain’s Decision Circuits  Silvia U.
Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
Single-Neuron Correlates of Atypical Face Processing in Autism
Bram-Ernst Verhoef, Rufin Vogels, Peter Janssen  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Ranulfo Romo, Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos  Neuron 
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules
Alan N. Hampton, Ralph Adolphs, J. Michael Tyszka, John P. O'Doherty 
Sang Wan Lee, Shinsuke Shimojo, John P. O’Doherty  Neuron 
Daphna Shohamy, Anthony D. Wagner  Neuron 
Marcus Grueschow, Rafael Polania, Todd A. Hare, Christian C. Ruff 
Christiane M Thiel, Karl J Friston, Raymond J Dolan  Neuron 
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Sheng Li, Stephen D. Mayhew, Zoe Kourtzi  Neuron 
Nils Kolling, Marco Wittmann, Matthew F.S. Rushworth  Neuron 
Perceptual Learning and Decision-Making in Human Medial Frontal Cortex
Dissociable Effects of Dopamine and Serotonin on Reversal Learning
Volume 93, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Volume 65, Issue 1, Pages (January 2010)
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Volume 71, Issue 4, Pages (August 2011)
Jeffrey Cockburn, Anne G.E. Collins, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Volume 65, Issue 6, Pages (March 2010)
Corticostriatal Output Gating during Selection from Working Memory
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Peter Kok, Janneke F.M. Jehee, Floris P. de Lange  Neuron 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
A. Saez, M. Rigotti, S. Ostojic, S. Fusi, C.D. Salzman  Neuron 
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Prefrontal Cortex Activity Related to Abstract Response Strategies
Caleb E. Strait, Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden  Neuron 
Fast Sequences of Non-spatial State Representations in Humans
Erie D. Boorman, John P. O’Doherty, Ralph Adolphs, Antonio Rangel 
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Volume 88, Issue 4, Pages (November 2015)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Michael J. Frank, Brion S. Woroch, Tim Curran  Neuron 
Megan E. Speer, Jamil P. Bhanji, Mauricio R. Delgado  Neuron 
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Multisensory Integration and Attention in Developmental Dyslexia
Volume 87, Issue 6, Pages (September 2015)
Volume 92, Issue 2, Pages (October 2016)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
David Badre, Bradley B. Doll, Nicole M. Long, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Cortical Signals for Rewarded Actions and Strategic Exploration
Perceptual Classification in a Rapidly Changing Environment
An Upside to Reward Sensitivity: The Hippocampus Supports Enhanced Reinforcement Learning in Adolescence  Juliet Y. Davidow, Karin Foerde, Adriana Galván,
Acute Stress Impairs Self-Control in Goal-Directed Choice by Altering Multiple Functional Connections within the Brain’s Decision Circuits  Silvia U.
Similarity Breeds Proximity: Pattern Similarity within and across Contexts Is Related to Later Mnemonic Judgments of Temporal Proximity  Youssef Ezzyat,
Mood as Representation of Momentum
Striatal Activity Underlies Novelty-Based Choice in Humans
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Volume 90, Issue 5, Pages (June 2016)
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Presentation transcript:

Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages 914-919 (November 2013) Disruption of Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Decreases Model-Based in Favor of Model- free Control in Humans  Peter Smittenaar, Thomas H.B. FitzGerald, Vincenzo Romei, Nicholas D. Wright, Raymond J. Dolan  Neuron  Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages 914-919 (November 2013) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.009 Copyright © 2013 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Task Design (A) On each trial, a choice between two stimuli led probabilistically to one of two further pairs of stimuli, which then demanded another choice followed by reward or no reward according to the p(reward) of the chosen second-stage stimulus that fluctuated over time. Importantly, participants could learn that each first-stage stimulus led more often (70%/30%) to one of the pairs; this task structure could then be exploited by a model-based, but not by a model-free, controller. (B) Model-based and model-free strategies for reinforcement learning predict differences in feedback processing particularly after uncommon transitions. If choices were exclusively model-free, then a reward would increase the likelihood of staying with the same stimulus on the next trial, regardless of the type of transition (left). Alternatively, if choices were driven by a model-based system, the impact of reward would interact with the transition type (middle). As shown previously, behavior in healthy participants resembles a hybrid of model-based and model-free control (right; Daw et al., 2011; Otto et al., 2013; Wunderlich et al., 2012b). We can thus quantify model-free control by estimating the main effect of reward, and model-based control by estimating the reward-by-transition interaction. Please see Figure S1 for a validation of the random walks. Neuron 2013 80, 914-919DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.009) Copyright © 2013 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Results (A) The probability of repeating the same first-stage choice is shown as a function of reward and transition experienced on the previous trial. The pattern of choices qualitatively resembles influences of both model-based and model-free control for all three stimulation sites (cf. Figure 1B, right). (B) We quantified model-free and model-based control as the main effect of reward and the reward-by-transition interaction, respectively, in a hierarchical logistic regression on stay/switch behavior on each trial. Disruption of right dlPFC reduced model-based control compared to vertex. TBS did not significantly affect model-free control. (C) The relative balance between the controllers was calculated as βmodel-based – βmodel-free. The balance significantly shifted toward model-free control after disruption of right, but not left, dlPFC compared to vertex. Error bars indicate SEM. Please see Figure S2 for additional stay-switch analyses. Neuron 2013 80, 914-919DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.009) Copyright © 2013 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Working Memory Capacity Interacts with Stimulation in Left dlPFC Working memory (WM) capacity did not predict the balance between model-based and model-free control after disruption of vertex (left) or right dlPFC (right). In contrast, higher WM was associated with relatively stronger model-based control after disruption of left dlPFC (middle) with the correlation being significantly more positive than for right dlPFC (permutation test, p = 0.009) or vertex (p = 0.06). Neuron 2013 80, 914-919DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.009) Copyright © 2013 The Authors Terms and Conditions