The visual perception of motion

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Biological Bifocal Lenses with Image Separation
Advertisements

Biological Optics: Deep Reflections
Wilhelm Hofmeister and the foundations of plant science
Stereopsis: How the brain sees depth
Prey–Predator Communication: For Your Sensors Only
Wilhelm Hofmeister and the foundations of plant science
Time perception: Brain time or event time?
Animal Vision: Rats Watch the Sky
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Jennifer Wathan, Karen McComb  Current Biology 
Natalia Zaretskaya, Andreas Bartels  Current Biology 
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
A new galloping gait in an insect
Vision: In the Brain of the Beholder
Insect Neurobiology: An Eye to Forward Motion
Biological Bifocal Lenses with Image Separation
Neurophysiology: Vertigo in MRI Machines
Infant cognition Current Biology
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
Visual Perception: Lightness in a High-Dynamic-Range World
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down
Robert J. Snowden, Alan B. Milne  Current Biology 
There's more to magic than meets the eye
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages R329-R331 (April 2018)
Face Perception: Broken into Parts
Immediate susceptibility to visual illusions after sight onset
Nocturnal Vision: Bees in the Dark
Vision: When Does Looking Bigger Mean Seeing Better?
A vast 4,000-year-old spatial pattern of termite mounds
Motion Processing: How Low Can You Go?
Jennifer Wathan, Karen McComb  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Motion vision: Are ‘speed lines’ used in human visual motion?
Bacterial Gliding Motility: Rolling Out a Consensus Model
Embryonic Axes: The Long and Short of It in the Mouse
Insect Vision: Controlling Actions through Optic Flow
Acrosomal Actin: Twists and Turns of a Versatile Filament
Volume 27, Issue 13, Pages R631-R636 (July 2017)
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages R963-R965 (November 2013)
Time perception: Brain time or event time?
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages R188-R189 (March 2015)
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Martijn Barendregt, Ben M. Harvey, Bas Rokers, Serge O. Dumoulin 
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Path Integration: Combining Optic Flow with Compass Orientation
Auditory perception: The near and far of sound localization
Motor Control: No Constant but Change
Volume 7, Issue 6, Pages R147-R151 (June 2000)
Albert V. van den Berg, Jaap A. Beintema  Neuron 
Binocular Vision: The Eyes Add and Subtract
Visual Perception: A Novel Difference Channel in Binocular Vision
Visual Circuits: Division of Labor Revealed
Insect Vision: Judging Distance with Binocular Motion Disparities
Perceptual Learning: Is V1 up to the Task?
Optimizing Visual Motion Perception during Eye Movements
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Basal bodies Current Biology
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Motor Control: Spinal Circuits Help Tadpoles See Clearly
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
14. The Duck-Rabbit Illusion
Presentation transcript:

The visual perception of motion Robert J. Snowden, Tom C.A. Freeman  Current Biology  Volume 14, Issue 19, Pages R828-R831 (October 2004) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.033

Figure 1 (A) The aperture problem. Consider a diamond moving to the right (left side of figure) or upwards (right side). Its position is illustrated at two points in time. Within the small receptive fields – apertures – the motion is identical for the two very different movements of the diamond. (B) A static illustration of the barberpole illusion. Current Biology 2004 14, R828-R831DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.033)

Figure 2 The optic-flow pattern when moving in a straight line across an evenly textured ground-plane. The square indicates the direction of heading. All image movement radiates away from this point with a speed that depends on the distance of the texture element from the eye. Current Biology 2004 14, R828-R831DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.033)

Figure 3 The illusion ‘Candies2’ by the artist Akiyoshi Kitaoka (© 2002). If you move your head backwards and forwards you should find that the outer circle of elements rotate in one direction, whilst the inner circles rotate in the opposite direction. The rotations will alternate depending on whether you move towards or away from the picture. See text for a possible explanation. Current Biology 2004 14, R828-R831DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.09.033)