How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Harold A. Burgess, Hannah Schoch, Michael Granato  Current Biology 
Advertisements

Volume 24, Issue 12, Pages (June 2014)
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 25, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
A Sparse Object Coding Scheme in Area V4
Volume 25, Issue 3, Pages (February 2015)
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Depth Affects Where We Look
Andres Laan, Tamar Gutnick, Michael J. Kuba, Gilles Laurent 
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
Real-Time Visualization of Neuronal Activity during Perception
Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Reconstitution of Amoeboid Motility In Vitro Identifies a Motor-Independent Mechanism for Cell Body Retraction  Katsuya Shimabukuro, Naoki Noda, Murray.
A Magnetic Map Leads Juvenile European Eels to the Gulf Stream
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Box Jellyfish Use Terrestrial Visual Cues for Navigation
Volume 25, Issue 13, Pages (June 2015)
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Susanne Åkesson, Jens Morin, Rachel Muheim, Ulf Ottosson 
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Effect of Expected Reward Magnitude on the Response of Neurons in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque  Matthew I. Leon, Michael N. Shadlen 
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Volume 78, Issue 5, Pages (June 2013)
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages e7 (September 2017)
Reconstitution of Amoeboid Motility In Vitro Identifies a Motor-Independent Mechanism for Cell Body Retraction  Katsuya Shimabukuro, Naoki Noda, Murray.
Cornelia Buehlmann, Paul Graham, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden 
The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration
Volume 111, Issue 7, Pages (October 2016)
Non-overlapping Neural Networks in Hydra vulgaris
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Volume 26, Issue 5, Pages (March 2016)
Communal Nutrition in Ants
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Tracking Route Progression in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
Michael L. Walls, John E. Layne  Current Biology 
Adrienn G. Varga, Roy E. Ritzmann  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 24, Pages R1083-R1085 (December 2013)
Path Integration: Combining Optic Flow with Compass Orientation
Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon 
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Ian C. Fiebelkorn, Yuri B. Saalmann, Sabine Kastner  Current Biology 
When Correlation Implies Causation in Multisensory Integration
Fruit-Catching Fish Tune Their Fast Starts to Compensate for Drift
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Calibration of vector navigation in desert ants
Metacognitive Failure as a Feature of Those Holding Radical Beliefs
Binaral Rivalry between the Nostrils and in the Cortex
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward Sebastian Schwarz, Michael Mangan, Jochen Zeil, Barbara Webb, Antoine Wystrach  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages 401-407 (February 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.019 Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Differences between Forward- and Backward-Walking Ants (A) Schematic aerial view of the experimental setup. Ants were constrained within a one-way foraging route between the nest and feeder (F). Gray lines within the route depict baffles, and gray goniometer sketches indicate test locations. Arrowheads indicate the theoretical directions tested in (B)–(D), as indicated by the path integration vector (black) and the correct route direction (gray). Note that backward zero-vector (ZV) ants appeared to be oriented toward the feeder-to-nest compass direction (open arrowhead). Dashed arrows indicate capture (CP) or release (RP) points, and the panoramic images illustrate the scenery, as perceived from the RPs. Images were taken with a Sony Bloggie, unwrapped and processed as in [6]. RP1: lower image represents view before baffle and upper image view after baffle. RP2,3: lower and upper images represent views with and without lampshade. Note that the sky was still visible even with the lampshade in place; only terrestrial visual cues were covered. (B) Circular distribution of heading directions (after 40 cm of travel) of full-vector (FV) ants tested at RP1. Forward ants were oriented and headed toward the route direction (gray arrowhead). Backward ants were oriented and headed along their path integration vector (black arrowhead). (C and D) Distributions of headings of ZV ants released on-route at RP2 (C) and off-route at RP3 (D). Forward ants showed random headings at 10 cm (lampshade) but were oriented toward the route direction (RP2) or nest direction (RP3) (gray arrowhead) at 40 cm. Backward ants were oriented toward the feeder-nest direction (open arrowhead) at 10 cm (only D) and 40 cm. (B–D) Arrows indicate mean vector and black circles indicated by the 95% confidence intervals of group distributions. Numbers in circular sectors indicate the number of observations for this direction. Filled stars within the histograms depict significantly oriented distributions (p < 0.01; Rayleigh test). Open stars near theoretical directions (arrowheads) indicate p < 0.001 against this direction as mean of the group distribution (S test, a non-parametric sign test for circular data, MATLAB). Filled stars outside the histograms depict significant differences (p < 0.001; WW test) between mean directions of two given distributions. Details on p values are provided in Table S1; see also Supplemental Experimental Procedures, Figure S1, and Movie S1. Current Biology 2017 27, 401-407DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.019) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Peeking Behavior Enables Ants to Adjust Their Direction of Travel Angular error (angle away from the homing direction) of the direction of backward travel 10 cm before and 10 cm after the peeking behavior. “Forward during peek” corresponds to the direction of the furthest location away from the dropped cookie reached by the ant during peeking behavior. Each line corresponds to an individual ant (p values correspond to a paired sign tests). Circular histograms show travel directions relative to the route direction (arrowhead), with mean vector of the distribution (arrow) and 95% confidence intervals (black circles). Numbers in circular sectors correspond to the number of observations for this direction. Filled stars within the histograms depict significantly oriented distributions (Rayleigh tests: before peek, p = 0.159; peek, p < 0.001; after peek, p < 0.001). Lower sketches illustrate an example of a peeking behavior sequence (dashed arrows depict direction of travel; see also Movie S2). Current Biology 2017 27, 401-407DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.019) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Ants Can Maintain a Direction of Travel Independently of their Body Orientation (A) Mirror experiment. Recorded paths were digitized and the ant’s location was extracted at two frames/s. Dashed lines represent path sections under direct (natural) sun conditions, and solid lines represent path sections with the sun mirrored by 180° compared to the ant position. Small arrows indicate the direction of the sun’s position in the sky, and black circles the start of the paths. The mirror manipulation was applied in backward-walking full-vector ants (vector direction indicated by open arrowhead) and zero-vector ants before or after the ant had displayed a peeking behavior. The circular histograms show the relative distribution of the travel direction of path segments sampled at two frames/s under direct natural sun (gray) and mirrored sun (white). The two associated vectors indicate the mean vector of the circular distributions; the x and y axis length indicate a vector norm of 1. (B) The angle turned by each individual (gray dots) is greater as a response to the manipulative change in sun direction than it is before or after the change, indicating the use of a celestial compass. As depicted, individual paths were divided into four successive vectors of 8 cm each: two before the manipulation (b2, b1) and two after (a1, a2). Angles turned correspond to the absolute angular difference between the vectors (before: |θb2 b1|; during: |θb1 a1|; after: |θa1 a2|). ∗p < 0.015, nonparametric “sign test” test (MATLAB) for paired individual data. (C) Example paths of single ants, traveling forward (with a small food item), backward, or in a combination of different body orientations (“mixed,” with a big food item). Head position (black dots) and head-to-tail orientation (black dashes) were extracted from the recorded paths (GoPro cameras) at five frames/s. Circular histograms show distributions of the ants’ body orientations relative to their direction of travel (travel direction – body orientation), computed as the change in location of the ants’ head from the current frame to the next (i.e., backward: BWD; forward: FWD; sideways: SWD; see also Movie S3). No correlation between body orientation and direction of travel could be found for the mixed ants (circular-circular correlation, 0.13 < r < −0.11, p > 0.324). (D) Distributions of directions of travel (path segments sampled at five frames/s of mixed ants shown in Figure 3C) for different categories of body orientations show that ants can maintain an overall path direction (aligned at zero) independently of their body orientation. Dashed lines indicate the mean direction expected if the ants were pulling backward. Filled and open dots indicate the means for the FV and ZV ants, respectively (see also Figure S2 for a similar analysis and result with Myrmecia data). Current Biology 2017 27, 401-407DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.019) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions