Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Advertisements

Cochlear implants Current Biology
Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
A Multisensory Network for Olfactory Processing
Wei-Hsiang Lin, Edo Kussell  Current Biology 
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Victoria F. Ratcliffe, David Reby  Current Biology 
Illusory Jitter Perceived at the Frequency of Alpha Oscillations
Maternal Regulation of Infant Brain State
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Sizing up dogs Current Biology
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages (June 2014)
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Chimpanzees Trust Their Friends
Keeping up with Bats: Dynamic Auditory Tuning in a Moth
Volume 20, Issue 6, Pages R282-R284 (March 2010)
Injecting Instructions into Premotor Cortex
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 26, Issue 8, Pages (April 2016)
Evolution of human vocal production
Jason Samaha, Bradley R. Postle  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Volume 18, Issue 11, Pages R453-R455 (June 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages e5 (January 2018)
Sounds of Modified Flight Feathers Reliably Signal Danger in a Pigeon
Volume 28, Issue 5, Pages e3 (March 2018)
Cochlear implants Current Biology
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Evolution of Eukaryotic DNA Methylation and the Pursuit of Safer Sex
Vocal Development: How Marmoset Infants Express Their Feelings
Perception Matches Selectivity in the Human Anterior Color Center
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Attentive Tracking of Sound Sources
Newborns' Cry Melody Is Shaped by Their Native Language
Asif A. Ghazanfar, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, W. Tecumseh Fitch 
Nicolas Mathevon, Caroline Casey, Colleen Reichmuth, Isabelle Charrier 
Direct Two-Dimensional Access to the Spatial Location of Covert Attention in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex  Elaine Astrand, Claire Wardak, Pierre Baraduc,
Volume 24, Issue 14, Pages (July 2014)
Janina Hesse, Susanne Schreiber  Current Biology 
Peng Zhang, Min Bao, Miyoung Kwon, Sheng He, Stephen A. Engel 
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Auditory-Visual Crossmodal Integration in Perception of Face Gender
Raghav Rajan, Allison J. Doupe  Current Biology 
Function and Evolution of Vibrato-like Frequency Modulation in Mammals
Zuzana Burivalova, Çağan Hakkı Şekercioğlu, Lian Pin Koh 
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Akinori Mitani, Mingyuan Dong, Takaki Komiyama  Current Biology 
Knowledgeable Lemurs Become More Central in Social Networks
Context-Dependent Functions of Avian Duets Revealed by Microphone-Array Recordings and Multispeaker Playback  Daniel J. Mennill, Sandra L. Vehrencamp 
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Cécile Fruteau, Eric van Damme, Ronald Noë  Current Biology 
Marie P. Suver, Akira Mamiya, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys
Søren K. Andersen, Steven A. Hillyard, Matthias M. Müller 
Volume 29, Issue 3, Pages e4 (February 2019)
Synergistic Effects of Marine Reserves and Harvest Controls on the Abundance and Catch Dynamics of a Coral Reef Fishery  Jess K. Hopf, Geoffrey P. Jones,
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e4 (September 2017)
Presentation transcript:

Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys Daniel Y. Takahashi, Diana A. Liao, Asif A. Ghazanfar  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 12, Pages 1844-1852.e6 (June 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Design and Potential Acoustic Parameters (A) Infants were briefly separated from their parents and placed in an acoustically treated testing room. Computer-controlled playbacks were delivered through a speaker. Sessions lasted ∼40 min, with the first 5 min (postnatal days 1 to 7) or 10 min used to collect spontaneous vocalizations. (B) Twin infants received either high-contingency playbacks (100%) or low-contingency playbacks (10%). Spectrograms depict when such playbacks (green) were delivered relative to the infant vocalizations. Warmer colors indicate higher values. (C) Four acoustic parameters change over the course of marmoset vocal development and were measured in the study: duration, dominant frequency, Wiener entropy, and amplitude modulation (AM) frequency. Vertical red dashed lines in spectrograms indicate the time interval used for the analyses in the neighboring panels. (D) Four acoustic parameters are related to different operations of the vocal apparatus (vocal tract, vocal folds, and lungs/respiration). Wiener entropy and AM frequency changes are associated with changes in the shape of upper vocal tract, vocal fold tension, and respiratory control. Change in dominant frequency is associated with size of upper vocal tract. Change in duration is associated with change in lung capacity and respiratory control. See also Figure S1. Current Biology 2017 27, 1844-1852.e6DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Infants Receiving More Contingent Vocal Feedback Develop Their Vocalizations Faster, and This Change Is Not Related to Growth Differences (A and B) Wiener entropy (in decibels) changes over postnatal days for high- and low-contingency infants. (A) shows group average; shaded regions indicate 1 SE intervals. (B) shows data for each twin set. (C and D) AM frequency (in Hz) changes over postnatal days for high- and low-contingency infants. (C) shows group average; (D) shows data for each twin set. (E) Growth of all infants as measured by weight (in g) over postnatal days. See also Figures S2 and S3. Current Biology 2017 27, 1844-1852.e6DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Call Duration and Dominant Frequency Are Not Influenced by the Amount of Vocal Feedback (A and B) Duration (in s) changes over postnatal days for high- and low-contingency infants. (A) shows group average; shaded regions indicate 1 SE intervals. (B) shows data for each twin set. (C and D) Dominant frequency (in kHz) changes over postnatal days for high- and low-contingency infants. (C) shows group average; (D) shows data for each twin set. See also Figures S2 and S3. Current Biology 2017 27, 1844-1852.e6DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Development of the Rate of Call Production Measured during the Baseline Interval (A) Rate of call production from postnatal day 1 to postnatal day 35 for each infant. Orange and black lines are cubic spline curves fitted to high- and low-contingency data, respectively. (B) Rate of call production from postnatal day 36 to postnatal day 60 for each infant. Orange and black lines are cubic spline curves fitted to high- and low-contingency data, respectively. Current Biology 2017 27, 1844-1852.e6DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions