Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Advertisements

Pea Plants Show Risk Sensitivity
Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1
Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Enhanced Muscle Afferent Signals during Motor Learning in Humans
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Sergei Gepshtein, Martin S. Banks  Current Biology 
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages e3 (September 2017)
Volume 64, Issue 4, Pages (November 2009)
Yukiyasu Kamitani, Frank Tong  Current Biology 
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Inactivation of Medial Frontal Cortex Changes Risk Preference
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Ryan T. Roemmich, Andrew W. Long, Amy J. Bastian  Current Biology 
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Motor Learning with Unstable Neural Representations
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Action Selection and Action Value in Frontal-Striatal Circuits
Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages e7 (September 2017)
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Rapid Automatic Motor Encoding of Competing Reach Options
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Bettina Sorger, Joel Reithler, Brigitte Dahmen, Rainer Goebel 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Motor Skills Are Strengthened through Reconsolidation
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Sam C. Berens, Jessica S. Horst, Chris M. Bird  Current Biology 
Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 21, Issue 11, Pages (June 2011)
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Context-Dependent Functions of Avian Duets Revealed by Microphone-Array Recordings and Multispeaker Playback  Daniel J. Mennill, Sandra L. Vehrencamp 
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages (November 2013)
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
J. Andrew Pruszynski, Roland S. Johansson, J. Randall Flanagan 
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Metacognitive Failure as a Feature of Those Holding Radical Beliefs
James N. Ingram, Ian S. Howard, J. Randall Flanagan, Daniel M. Wolpert 
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems
Presentation transcript:

The Value of the Follow-Through Derives from Motor Learning Depending on Future Actions  Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology  Volume 25, Issue 3, Pages 397-401 (February 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.037 Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Associating Different Follow-Through Movements with Motor Skills Reduces Interference (A) Participants made an initial movement to a central target (green circle). During exposure trials, a velocity-dependent curl force field (force vectors shown as blue arrows) was applied during this movement, and the field direction (clockwise [CW] or counter-clockwise [CCW]) was determined by a visual target location (T1 or T2). A follow-through group made a subsequent unperturbed movement to the target location, whereas a no-follow-through group remained at the central target. The directions of the force-field (CW or CCW) and follow-through movement (+45° or −45°) were counter-balanced across participants. Participants made movement in four directions but for clarity only one direction is shown. (B and C) The kinematic error (B) and force adaptation (C) (mean ± SE across participants for pairs of blocks, combining adjacent even and odd blocks) for the follow-through (brown) and no-follow-through (blue) groups. See also Figures S1 and S2 and Table S1. Current Biology 2015 25, 397-401DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.037) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Consistent Follow-Through Improves Learning Rate (A) Participants made a movement to a central target (green circle) followed by a follow-through movement to a target. During exposure trials, a curl force field was applied on the movement to the central target. The consistent-follow-through group always made the follow-through movement to the same target, whereas for the variable-follow-through group the target was randomly selected from nine possible locations on each trial. The direction of the force-field and follow-through movement was counter-balanced across participants. (B and C) The kinematic error (B) and force adaptation (C) (ten-trial running mean ± SE across participants) for consistent-follow-through (red) and variable-follow-through (blue) groups. Solid lines show fits of a dual-rate model to force compensation. There are 40 channel trials for each participant, plotted according to the trial number at which they were presented in a pseudo-random fashion. (D) Parameters of fits (with 95% confidence intervals) of the dual-rate model to both groups. See also Table S2. Current Biology 2015 25, 397-401DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.037) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Participants Show Extended Learning of Skills that Nonlineary Depend on Both Lead-In and Follow-Through (A) Participants made movements from one of two start locations to one of two target locations (four possible movements). Movements had to pass through two via points (V1 and V2). On each exposure trial, a force field was applied in the region between the via points (exposure region), and the field direction (CW or CCW) was uniquely specified by the combination of the start and target locations according to an XOR rule (table). The direction of the force field (CW or CCW) was counter-balanced across participants. (B and C) The kinematic error (B) and force adaptation (C) (mean ± SE across participants for pairs of blocks, combining adjacent even and odd blocks) over the 5 days of the experiment. (D) The force compensation averaged over the last 20 blocks (mean ± SE across participants) for each of the four movements, with dashed bars indicating full compensation. (E) Average of the variance explained by a regression analysis across participants. The regression analysis was used to predict the pattern of compensation (D) by fitting weights to the four possible patterns (bias, start, target, XOR) across the movements. The percentage shows the amount of the variance in force compensation explained by these four patterns (see the Supplemental Experimental Procedures for details). See also Figure S3. Current Biology 2015 25, 397-401DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.037) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions