Neuroscience: Intelligence in the Honeybee Mushroom Body

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Behavioral Genetics: Of Mice, Men, and Internal Bliss
Advertisements

Cuban efforts bolstered
Merging of Long-Term Memories in an Insect
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Neuroscience: What You See and Hear Is What You Get
Brain Plasticity: When the Feet and Mouth Replace the Hand
Laminopathies: Too Much SUN Is a Bad Thing
Nuclear envelope Current Biology
Human Memory: Brain-State-Dependent Effects of Stimulation
Decision Making: How the Brain Weighs the Evidence
Volume 18, Issue 22, Pages R1038-R1039 (November 2008)
Snub to poor dog breeding
Pericycle Current Biology
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Associative Learning: The Instructive Function of Biogenic Amines
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Cuban efforts bolstered
Microbiology: Mixing Wine, Chocolate, and Coffee
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Volume 16, Issue 8, Pages R265-R266 (April 2006)
Whistled Turkish alters language asymmetries
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Integrative Cell Biology: Katanin at the Crossroads
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Darwin’s meadow revisited
Cell Division: SACing the Anaphase Problem
Infant cognition Current Biology
Volume 25, Issue 24, Pages R1156-R1158 (December 2015)
Memory Processing: Ripples in the Resting Brain
Volume 24, Issue 5, Pages R204-R206 (March 2014)
Palaeontology: Scrapes of Dinosaur Courtship
Volume 20, Issue 12, Pages R500-R501 (June 2010)
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 19, Issue 3, Pages R92-R93 (February 2009)
Life History Evolution: What Does a Menopausal Killer Whale Do?
Social Learning: Public Information in Insects
Neuroscience: The Rhythms of Speech Understanding
Honeybee Communication: A Signal for Danger
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Volume 23, Issue 7, Pages R265-R266 (April 2013)
Better Fruits and Vegetables through Sensory Analysis
Brain Evolution: Getting Better All the Time?
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Dichoptic training enables the adult amblyopic brain to learn
Volume 24, Issue 7, Pages R262-R263 (March 2014)
It’s all about the constraints
Neuroscience: An Olfactory Homunculus in the Insect Brain
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Pericycle Current Biology
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 18, Issue 24, Pages R1115-R1116 (December 2008)
Volume 16, Issue 5, Pages R147-R151 (March 2006)
FOXO transcription factors
Feeding the future world
Volume 16, Issue 15, Pages R565-R566 (August 2006)
Volume 7, Issue 6, Pages R147-R151 (June 2000)
Retinal physiology: Adapting to the changing scene
Brain Stimulation: Feeling the Buzz
Volume 19, Issue 20, Pages R922-R923 (November 2009)
Volume 4, Issue 3, Pages (March 2018)
Sensory Evolution: Trouble in the Cherry Orchard
Evolution: A Paradigm Shift in Snake Sex Chromosome Genetics
Basal bodies Current Biology
Animal Personalities: The Advantage of Diversity
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Presentation transcript:

Neuroscience: Intelligence in the Honeybee Mushroom Body Sophie Caron, Larry F. Abbott  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages R220-R223 (March 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.011 Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Peak-shift in the honeybee mushroom body. The model of the honeybee mushroom body devised by Peng and Chittka [3] was trained with different learning paradigms. During an absolute learning task, the model learns to associate stimulus A (depicted as a red and blue flower) with a sugar reward. After training, the attraction of the modeled mushroom body peaks for the stimulus it was trained with (in this case, the red and blue flower). During a differential learning task, the model learns the same association as well as to associate stimulus B (depicted as a red flower) with a punishment. After training, the attraction of the modeled mushroom body peaks for a stimulus it was trained with (a blue flower). This phenomenon is called ‘peak-shift’ and is a form of learning that enables the brain to form inferences based on different past experiences. Current Biology 2017 27, R220-R223DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.011) Copyright © 2017 Terms and Conditions