Michael T. Rogan, Kam Sam Leon, David L. Perez, Eric R. Kandel  Neuron 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 86, Issue 5, Pages (June 2015)
Advertisements

Sniffing Behavior Communicates Social Hierarchy
Volume 83, Issue 2, Pages (July 2014)
Thomas Kantermann, Myriam Juda, Martha Merrow, Till Roenneberg 
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Mark E.J. Sheffield, Michael D. Adoff, Daniel A. Dombeck  Neuron 
Guangying K. Wu, Pingyang Li, Huizhong W. Tao, Li I. Zhang  Neuron 
Volume 84, Issue 5, Pages (December 2014)
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Endocannabinoids Control the Induction of Cerebellar LTD
Volume 77, Issue 5, Pages (March 2013)
Spike-Timing-Dependent Potentiation of Sensory Surround in the Somatosensory Cortex Is Facilitated by Deprivation-Mediated Disinhibition  Frédéric Gambino,
The Generation of Direction Selectivity in the Auditory System
Mismatch Receptive Fields in Mouse Visual Cortex
Christiane M Thiel, Karl J Friston, Raymond J Dolan  Neuron 
Jason N. MacLean, Brendon O. Watson, Gloster B. Aaron, Rafael Yuste 
Volume 67, Issue 6, Pages (September 2010)
Oded Klavir, Rotem Genud-Gabai, Rony Paz  Neuron 
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Jianrong Tang, John A. Dani  Neuron 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Volume 96, Issue 4, Pages e5 (November 2017)
Lior Cohen, Gideon Rothschild, Adi Mizrahi  Neuron 
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Selective Entrainment of Theta Oscillations in the Dorsal Stream Causally Enhances Auditory Working Memory Performance  Philippe Albouy, Aurélien Weiss,
Aryeh Hai Taub, Rita Perets, Eilat Kahana, Rony Paz  Neuron 
Tumor Necrosis Factor-α Mediates One Component of Competitive, Experience- Dependent Plasticity in Developing Visual Cortex  Megumi Kaneko, David Stellwagen,
A Map for Horizontal Disparity in Monkey V2
New Experiences Enhance Coordinated Neural Activity in the Hippocampus
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Hippocampal “Time Cells”: Time versus Path Integration
Spike Timing-Dependent LTP/LTD Mediates Visual Experience-Dependent Plasticity in a Developing Retinotectal System  Yangling Mu, Mu-ming Poo  Neuron 
SK2 Channel Modulation Contributes to Compartment-Specific Dendritic Plasticity in Cerebellar Purkinje Cells  Gen Ohtsuki, Claire Piochon, John P. Adelman,
Ju Tian, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 80, Issue 2, Pages (October 2013)
Volume 84, Issue 5, Pages (December 2014)
Volume 87, Issue 2, Pages (July 2015)
Jason N. MacLean, Brendon O. Watson, Gloster B. Aaron, Rafael Yuste 
Georg B. Keller, Tobias Bonhoeffer, Mark Hübener  Neuron 
Feng Han, Natalia Caporale, Yang Dan  Neuron 
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Timing, Timing, Timing: Fast Decoding of Object Information from Intracranial Field Potentials in Human Visual Cortex  Hesheng Liu, Yigal Agam, Joseph.
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
An Animal Model of a Behavioral Intervention for Depression
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Posterior Parietal Cortex Encodes Autonomously Selected Motor Plans
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Two Directions of Plasticity in the Sensory-Deprived Adult Cortex
Volume 53, Issue 5, Pages (March 2007)
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum  John P. O'Doherty, Tony W.
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 83, Issue 4, Pages (August 2014)
Dopamine-Dependent Interactions between Limbic and Prefrontal Cortical Plasticity in the Nucleus Accumbens: Disruption by Cocaine Sensitization  Yukiori.
Role of the Cerebellum in Adaptation to Delayed Action Effects
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Volume 67, Issue 6, Pages (September 2010)
Supratim Ray, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Yuko Yotsumoto, Takeo Watanabe, Yuka Sasaki  Neuron 
Irreplaceability of Neuronal Ensembles after Memory Allocation
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Thomas Kantermann, Myriam Juda, Martha Merrow, Till Roenneberg 
Volume 37, Issue 3, Pages (February 2003)
Presentation transcript:

Distinct Neural Signatures for Safety and Danger in the Amygdala and Striatum of the Mouse  Michael T. Rogan, Kam Sam Leon, David L. Perez, Eric R. Kandel  Neuron  Volume 46, Issue 2, Pages 309-320 (April 2005) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017 Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Conditioned Safety and Conditioned Fear in the Mouse The bars show the mean percentage of time spent freezing (defensive tonic immobility) during 20 s CS (black) and 20 s prior to CS (context: white). Before safety conditioning (A) or fear conditioning (B), the CS is neutral and does not alter behavior (days 1 and 2). Behavior during training (gray: [A] and [B], days 3 and 4) is dominated by response to US delivery (Phillips and LeDoux, 1992), and is therefore not included in the statistical analysis. Upon return to the conditioning box after safety conditioning with unpaired CS and US ([A], days 5 and 6), mice displayed fear responses to the experimental context, which invariably accrue with US exposure. However, this level of conditioned fear is significantly reduced by the arrival of the safety CS. The small but significant level of freezing displayed during the CS largely reflects the brief persistence of freezing from the pre-CS period into the CS period. After fear conditioning, the mouse show less fear of the context than mice receiving unpaired CS US training, which is a well-known phenomenon ([B], days 5 and 6). The arrival of the fear CS significantly increases the level of freezing. (C) Separate groups of mice received 2 days (data not shown) of either safety conditioning (n = 8) or CS tone control (n = 8). Mice were then fear conditioned in a novel context with two CS/US pairings per day over 4 days. The bars show mean percentage of freezing over the 20 s pre-CS and CS periods prior to the first US on each day. The CS tone control shows normal fear conditioning acquisition on day 2, reaching a significant increase in freezing to the CS tone, with respect to both pretraining baseline (day 1) and the pre-CS period (adjacent white bar) after 1 training day. However, fear conditioning with the safety CS was significantly retarded compared to the CS tone control (day 2). The previously safety conditioned CS did not increase freezing beyond the pre-CS level until day 4. The data shown in (A) and (B) were acquired during electrophysiological recording (see Figures 4–6), collapsed across recording site; unpaired: n = 17; paired: n = 19. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with group as between-subject factor and day (1, 2, 5, 6) and stimulus (pre-cs, cs) as repeated measures reveal a significant main effect of group (F(1,34) = 16.40, p < 0.001) and a significant interaction of stimulus × group (F(1,34) = 495.49, p < 0.001). Post hoc comparison of stimulus (pre-cs, cs) within day yielded significant differences (Fisher’s PLSD, p < 0.001) and are noted by *. For data in Figure 2C, two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with group as between-subject factor and day and stimulus (pre-cs, cs) as repeated measures reveal a significant interaction of stimulus × group (F(1,14) = 13.14, p < 0.003). Post hoc comparison of stimulus (pre-cs, cs) within day and across groups yielded significant differences (Fisher’s PLSD, p < 0.05) and are noted by *. All error bars are ± SEM. Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Learned and Unlearned Safety Signals Release Exploratory Behavior (A) and (B) show the paths taken by representative mice in an open field during 1 min periods (left). On the right, the same data are expressed as the probability that the mouse occupied each point along the radial distance from the wall of the arena. The gray line delineates a simulated “random walk” within the field. (A) A “handled only” mouse stayed close to the wall both before (min 1) and during (min 2) the neutral CS tone. (B) A safety conditioned mouse also stays close to the wall in min 1, but during the safety CS (green, min 2) moves toward the center of the open field. (C) (Top) All groups travel less distance in min 2 except in the presence of the learned safety signal (green, Safety trained) or the instinctive safety signal (reduced ambient lighting: Dimmer). During the fear CS, mice display a combination of freezing and rapid darting runs that averages to a mean distance similar to that displayed by other groups. ANOVA for trained groups: F(1,35) = 52.4, p < 0.001; ANOVA for untrained groups: F(1,52) = 5.2, p < 0.001. (C) (Middle) Both learned and instinctive safety signals increase the percentage distance traveled in the center zone and the percentage time spent in center (data not shown). ANOVA for trained groups: F(1,35) = 52.4, p < 0.01; ANOVA for untrained groups: F(1,52) = 5.2, p < 0.01. (C) (Bottom) Mean radial distance from the wall of field toward the center is increased only by safety signals. This continuous measure is free from arbitrary definition of a “center zone.” ANOVA for trained groups: F(1,34) = 6.15, p < 0.0001; ANOVA for untrained groups: F(1,52) = 4.15, p < 0.005. For all groups: Fisher’s post hoc comparisons, *between groups p < 0.01; within group, min 1 versus min 2, p < 0.05. Error bars are ± SEM. Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Safety Conditioned Mice Display a Preference for the Safety CS in Neutral Conditions After 2 days of safety conditioning (n = 7, filled circle) or CS tone control (n = 8, open square), mice were placed in a rectangular arena consisting of two adjacent rooms connected by an open doorway. (A) The path taken by a representative animal during the first 5 min (no CS) shows no thigmotaxis and no room preference. (B) Group data show the mean percentage of time per minute spent by mice in the room designated for pairing with the CS. During training and testing sessions, this is equivalent to the mean percentage of time per minute that the animal listened to the CS. No preference was shown for either room in the absence of the CS (first 5 min). During training, all mice crossed the threshold repeatedly, and there was no significant difference in the number of threshold crossings (i.e., exposure to the operant contingency) between groups (CS tone control: mean 21.40, 6.00 SD; Safety conditioned: mean 16.71, 5.4 SD; t test p > 0.05; data not shown). On the following day, safety trained mice preferred the CS room. The CS tone controls showed no room preference at any time. Repeated-measures ANOVA with group as between-subjects factor and minute as a repeated measure yielded a main effect of group (F(1,13) = 6.08; p < 0.001) and a significant minute × group interaction (F(2,26) = 5.17; p < 0.05). Fisher’s post hoc comparisons: *between groups p < 0.05. Error bars are ± SEM. Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Recording Locations within the Caudoputamen and the Lateral Amygdala Histological reconstruction of recording electrode positions during fear conditioning (triangle), safety conditioning (circle), and CS tone control (cross). (Modified from Paxinos and Franklin, 2001). Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 CS Processing in the Lateral Amygdala (A) Transmission speeds of auditory CS information via thalamic and thalamocortical routes to LA. Since our goal was to establish the latency of the first arriving CS information at LA, we report the fastest multiunit latencies observed. (B) CS-evoked field potential (top) and multiunit activity (bottom) recorded in LA (mean of 20 tone pip responses). Shaded bars indicate the arrival times of the earliest CS information relayed to LA via a direct MGm/PIN-LA pathway (light gray) and routed through auditory cortex (dark gray). (C) Representative CS-evoked field potentials (mean of five CS presentations) 1 day before (black) and 1 day after 2 days of fear conditioning (red) or safety conditioning (green). CS-evoked field potentials were quantified by measuring the latency from CS onset, slope, and amplitude of the negative-going potential (marked with circle) identified in acute studies (see [B]). (D) Group data of slope and amplitude of CS-evoked field potentials, normalized as the percentage of mean baseline measures (see Figure 1 for concurrent behavioral measures). Each point represents the mean of five CS presentations for that day. Safety conditioning (green, n = 8) decreased LA CS-evoked field potential slope and amplitude compared to pretraining baseline and to the CS tone controls (black, n = 7). In contrast, fear conditioning (red, n = 9) increases LA CS-evoked field potential slope and amplitude compared to pretraining baseline and to CS tone controls. The CS tone controls did not change significantly over the course of the experiment. The normalized slope and amplitude were evaluated statistically with two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with group (safety, fear, CS tone control) as the between-subjects factor and experimental session as repeated measure. A significant group-session interaction was observed for both measures (slope, F(3, 63) = 6.292, p < 0.005; amplitude, F(3, 63) = 3.520, p < 0.005). A significant difference between fear conditioned or safety conditioned groups and CS tone controls for each session day is noted (#p < 0.05; +p < 0.07). A significant difference of training and posttraining percent changes from mean baseline is noted *t test, p < 0.05. Error bars are ± SEM. Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 CS Processing in the Caudoputamen The same measurements and analysis performed in LA (see Figure 5 legend) were performed in CP. (A) Fastest observed transmission speeds of auditory CS information via thalamic and thalamocortical routes to CP. (B) CS-evoked field potential (top) and multiunit activity recorded in CP (mean of 20 tone pip responses). Shaded bars indicate the arrival times of the earliest CS information relayed to CP via a direct MGm/PIN-CP pathway (light gray) and routed through auditory cortex (dark gray). (C) Representative CS-evoked field potentials 1 day before (black) and 1 day after 2 days of fear conditioning (red) or safety conditioning (green). (D) Group data of slope and amplitude of CS-evoked field potentials, normalized as a percentage of mean baseline measures (see Figure 1 for concurrent behavioral measures). Safety conditioning (green, n = 9) increases CP CS-evoked field potential slope and amplitude with respect to pretraining baseline and to CS tone controls (black, n = 6), while fear conditioning (red, n = 10) has no effect. The normalized slope and amplitude were evaluated statistically with two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs with group (fear, safety, CS tone controls) as the between-subjects factor and experimental session as repeated measure. A significant effect of session was observed for both measures (slope, F(2,66) = 26.95, p < 0.001; amplitude, F(2,66) = 16.52, p < 0.001). A significant difference between either fear conditioned or safety conditioned groups and the CS tone control for each session day is noted (#p < 0.001). A significant difference of training and posttraining percent changes from mean baseline within each group is noted *t test, p < 0.05. Error bars are ± SEM. Neuron 2005 46, 309-320DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.017) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions