From morpheme to utterance: A Morphosyntactic Approaching

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
CODE/ CODE SWITCHING.
Advertisements

C O N T E X T - F R E E LANGUAGES ( use a grammar to describe a language) 1.
Morphology.
The Study Of Language Unit 7 Presentation By: Elham Niakan Zahra Ghana’at Pisheh.
Greenberg 1963 Some Universals of Grammar with Particular Reference to the Order of Meaningful Elements.
Grammars.
Morphology Nuha Alwadaani.
Morphology Chapter 7 Prepared by Alaa Al Mohammadi.
Introduction to Linguistics n About how many words does the average 17 year old know?
Lecture -3 Week 3 Introduction to Linguistics – Level-5 MORPHOLOGY
Morphology.
The Eight Parts of Speech With Baseball Evan Gilman and Clayton Wilms.
Phonemes A phoneme is the smallest phonetic unit in a language that is capable of conveying a distinction in meaning. These units are identified within.
323 Morphology The Structure of Words 3. Lexicon and Rules 3.1 Productivity and the Lexicon The lexicon is in theory infinite, but in practice it is limited.
Morphology A Closer Look at Words By: Shaswar Kamal Mahmud.
Linguistics The ninth week. Chapter 3 Morphology  3.1 Introduction  3.2 Morphemes.
M ORPHOLOGY Lecturer/ Najla AlQahtani. W HAT IS MORPHOLOGY ? It is the study of the basic forms in a language. A morpheme is “a minimal unit of meaning.
Parts of Speech Major source: Wikipedia. Adjectives An adjective is a word that modifies a noun or a pronoun, usually by describing it or making its meaning.
Natural Language Processing Chapter 2 : Morphology.
MORPHOLOGY definition; variability among languages.
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
III. MORPHOLOGY. III. Morphology 1. Morphology The study of the internal structure of words and the rules by which words are formed. 1.1 Open classes.
3 Phonology: Speech Sounds as a System No language has all the speech sounds possible in human languages; each language contains a selection of the possible.
◦ Process of describing the structure of phrases and sentences Chapter 8 - Phrases and sentences: grammar1.
Slang. Informal verbal communication that is generally unacceptable for formal writing.
MORPHOLOGY. PART 1: INTRODUCTION Parts of speech 1. What is a part of speech?part of speech 1. Traditional grammar classifies words based on eight parts.
The structure and Function of Phrases and Sentences
Pronouns Pronouns are used in place of nouns, mostly to avoid repetition. Personal pronouns – refer to particular people: I, you, us. Impersonal pronouns.
Chapter 3 Word Formation I This chapter aims to analyze the morphological structures of words and gain a working knowledge of the different word forming.
INTRODUCTION ADE SUDIRMAN, S.Pd ENGLISH DEPARTMENT MATHLA’UL ANWAR UNIVERSITY.
Modeling Arithmetic, Computation, and Languages Mathematical Structures for Computer Science Chapter 8 Copyright © 2006 W.H. Freeman & Co.MSCS SlidesAlgebraic.
Lec. II The Morphology of English
Descriptive Grammar – 2S, 2016 Mrs. Belén Berríos
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
Morphological Types of Languages
Distributional analysis
Morphology Morphology Morphology Dr. Amal AlSaikhan Morphology.
Lecture -3 Week 3 Introduction to Linguistics – Level-5 MORPHOLOGY
Introduction to Linguistics
Context-Free Grammars: an overview
عمادة التعلم الإلكتروني والتعليم عن بعد
Revision Outcome 1, Unit 1 The Nature and Functions of Language
Chapter 2 Section 8.
GREEK ADJECTIVES
Chapter 3 Morphology Without grammar, little can be conveyed. Without vocabulary, nothing can be conveyed. (David Wilkins ,1972) Morphology refers to.
Classification of Languages
Lecturer Ms. Abrar Mujaddidi LANE 321
Chapter 6 Morphology.
Part I: Basics and Constituency
Word Classes and Affixes
Chapter 2 Section 8.
THE NATURE of LEARNER LANGUAGE
Structural relations Carnie 2013, chapter 4 Kofi K. Saah.
Syntax.
A Systematic Framework for Language Analysis
Natural Language - General
Chapter 1 Section 1.
CHAPTER 2 Context-Free Languages
Compilers Principles, Techniques, & Tools Taught by Jing Zhang
Língua Inglesa - Aspectos Morfossintáticos
Levels of Linguistic Analysis
Teori Bahasa dan Automata Lecture 9: Contex-Free Grammars
323 Morphology The Structure of Words 5
Copyright © Cengage Learning. All rights reserved.
Chhatrapati Shivaji College, Satara
Introduction to English morphology
Introduction to Linguistics
COMPILER CONSTRUCTION
Key Stage 1 Grammar.
Presentation transcript:

From morpheme to utterance: A Morphosyntactic Approaching

Introduction The-now-procedures The Harrisian proposal

The Harrisian Proposal The aim: compact procedure for describing utterances. Requirements: a list of morphemes and a sequence of morphemes The operation: substitution & repetition Final goal: a general (syntactic) formula applicable to each language yielding a compact statement of sequences of morphemes occurring in that particular language. The aim: formalizing a compact procedure for describing utterances in terms sequences of morphemes rather than single morphemes Requirements: a list of morphemes and a sequence of morphemes The operation: substitution & repetition Final goal: a formula applicable to languages yielding a compact statement of sequences of morphemes occurring in a particular language.

The rationale Syntax Explicitness Extension Explicitness: formal (of form) description , direct manipulation of observable morphemes Extension: substitution gliding from single morphemes to sequences of morphemes, and thus Syntax: syntactic analysis aims at a compact description of the structure of utterances in any given language, morphemes are ideal for syntactic procedures.

The proposal

The Proposal Assumption # 1 Complementary variants of a morpheme may be included in a single morpheme. e.g. The set /s, z, əz/ constitute three morphemes yet can be included in one morpheme namely, the plural ending of English -s

e.g. haiša haktana ( = the small woman) (האישה הקטנה) Assumption # 2 Repeated (identical ) affixation is structured as phrasal infixes. e.g. haiša haktana ( = the small woman) (האישה הקטנה) e.g. Una chica buena e.g. ∂lmullim∂h ∂lз∂meel∂h (المعلمة الجميلة) All these can be regarded not as word affixes or suffixes but rather a single phrase infx

The environment To come out with the generalization, we need to set the environment (frame), limitations of occurrence The fewer the limitations, the simpler the syntactic procedure (economy)

The operation List  Test  Class  Retest  (Subclass)  Generalize

How it works Morpheme Sequences (144) R, S, T can occur between the frame P ___ Q: (a) Separately: P _R_ Q (b) In a sequence which is taken to be a unit (here enclosed in square brackets): P _[RS]_ Q P [RST] Q In (b), ‘We may say that they are all members of one substitution class [i.e., a unit] which is now not merely a class of morphemes [e.g., ‘N’] but a class of morpheme sequences. the novelty of the Harrisian procedure is the extension of substitution classes to include sequences of morphemes. Not only A and B but also AE and even FGH are all substitutable members in a sequence if happen to occur in an A- B environment.

Formalizing the System English has these major morpheme classes (among others): T: Determiners; ‘T’ for ‘The’ and the like N: Nouns V: Verbs A: Adjectives D: Adverbs P: Prepositions

Hidatsa, a polysynthetic language, has these major morpheme classes (among others): S: Stem P: Prefixes to stems and other prefixes U: Utterance-final suffixes; ‘these occur at the end of (as well as within) within stretches of speech’ (149) F: ‘clause Final suffixes which can be substituted for U if the utterance continues’ (149) N: ‘Non-clause-final suffixes’ (149)

‘What sequences of morphemes can be substituted for single morphemes’ ‘Sequences of morphemes which are found to be substitutable in virtually all environments [frames] for some single morpheme class [e.g., N] will be equated to that class: AN = N’ (146) See page 146 §4.2 for exactly how to determine N1, N2, V1, V2, etc. To limit the generality of these substitutions (so that they are not too general), we use numerical superscripts: N1 + s = N2 Paper + s = papers. Since ‘papers’ is an N2, it can no longer be substituted for N1 (‘paper’).

Formalization (cont’d) ‘On the left-hand side of these equations, each raised number will be understood to include all lower numbers (unless otherwise noted) [. . .] On the right-hand side, however, each number includes itself alone’ (146). For example: TN2 = N3 is an abbreviation for both: TN1 = N3 and TN2 = N3

The higher the number, the more general the formula. This is because higher numbers can (but do not have to) include more single morphemes than lower numbers!! Affixes (147) Affixes never occur alone. So a V can be turned into an N with an affix, but the affix never appears on its own: ‘-Vn’ means: a suffix (‘-’ before ‘Vn’) attaching to a V (the big ‘V’ is what it attaches to) and creating an N (the little ‘n’ is what it creates). E.g., ‘-er’ in ‘writer’. ‘Nv-’ means: a prefix (‘-’ after ‘Nv’) attaching to an N and deriving a V. E.g., ‘en-’ in ‘enshrine’.

An Example from English TN2 = N3: ‘These pointless, completely transparent jokes’ Breakdown of this phrase, without left-hand abbreviations (see slide 16) and rearranged for clarity: T[N2] N2 = [N1 - s] (plural marker; 147 §4.33) N1 = [A2N1] = (147 §4.32) A2N1 = [A2A2N1] (147 §4.32, top right) A2 = [N - Na] (147 §4.33) A2 = [DA1] (147 §4.32, bottom left) D = [A1 - Ad] (147 §4.31, top left) With the colour coding, this sentence is: ‘These [point-less, [complete-ly transparent] joke-s]’ = N3 ☺

‘The great majority of English utterances’ (149) are at the most general level N4V4. ‘Most utterances of Hidatsa [. . .] consist of S4U’ (150 §5.3). The Theory Itself Position Analysis (and not Morpheme Analysis): ‘strict adherence to morpheme-distribution classes would lead to a relatively large number of different classes’ (150), e.g., think is V, house is N, but take is G (it forms a new class).

Variables then are positions, and not morphemes, to limit the number of classes. Thus ‘the element [i.e., morpheme] which occurs in a class position [e.g., V] may be a morpheme which occurs also in various other class positions [e.g., N, D, T, A, etc.]’ (150) ‘The final result for each language [. . .] takes the form of one or more substitution classes [e.g., S4U]. The formulae tell us that these are the sequences which occur’ (150) and that no other sequences may occur which are not ‘derived from the formula’ (150). ‘N has the [positional] values AN, TAN, TA, etc., and [in turn,] each of these [class positions] has specific morphemes as values’ (150)

Implicit in the Formulae Suprasegmental Features Morphological Boundaries Morphological [Syntactic] Relations: what affixes actually change the morpheme class; which affixes do not; etc. Endocentric/Exocentric relations (having a head/no head which determines the morpheme class: TN2 = N3/TA2 = N3). Order (the order in which morpheme classes come)

(Explicitly) Excluded from the Formulae The following ‘syntactic facts [. . .] cannot be included in [the formulas] and must be found by separate investigations and expressed in separate statements’ (152): Selection Features. These are (mostly) fine-tuned features within each class, creating sub-classes (wire-house/write-poetry/*‘wire-poetry’ in normal speech).

‘The formulae also cannot in themselves indicate what [semantic] meanings may be associated with the various positions or classes’ (152 §8.3). The formulas do not distinguish what an ‘N’ means from what a ‘V’ means, or subject from object, etc.; it is equally possible to include e.g., ‘◘’ (a completely meaningless symbol) for ‘V’, ‘◊’ for ‘N’. This is why the formulas are syntactic (the way the morphemes/words are strung together into patterns) without reference to their semantics (what the morpheme patterns actually mean).