Genetic Identification and Separation of Innate and Experience-Dependent Courtship Behaviors in Drosophila  Yufeng Pan, Bruce S. Baker  Cell  Volume 156,

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
transformer and Sex determination in Drosophila
Advertisements

Sex-Specific Aging in Flies, Worms, and Missing Great-Granddads
Are Complex Behaviors Specified by Dedicated Regulatory Genes
Abdominal-B Neurons Control Drosophila Virgin Female Receptivity
Cholinergic Control of Synchronized Seminal Emissions in Drosophila
Alessio Vagnoni, Simon L. Bullock  Current Biology 
J. Dylan Clyne, Gero Miesenböck  Cell 
Volume 61, Issue 4, Pages (February 2009)
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
Volume 19, Issue 12, Pages (June 2017)
Troy R. Shirangi, Allan M. Wong, James W. Truman, David L. Stern 
Activation of Latent Courtship Circuitry in the Brain of Drosophila Females Induces Male-like Behaviors  Carolina Rezával, Siddharth Pattnaik, Hania J.
Volume 53, Issue 1, Pages (January 2007)
Interspecies Sex and Taste
The Drosophila Female Aphrodisiac Pheromone Activates ppk23+ Sensory Neurons to Elicit Male Courtship Behavior  Hirofumi Toda, Xiaoliang Zhao, Barry J.
Volume 134, Issue 4, Pages (August 2008)
Starvation-Induced Depotentiation of Bitter Taste in Drosophila
Scribble Scaffolds a Signalosome for Active Forgetting
Representations of Taste Modality in the Drosophila Brain
Octopamine in Male Aggression of Drosophila
Opposing Dopaminergic and GABAergic Neurons Control the Duration and Persistence of Copulation in Drosophila  Michael A. Crickmore, Leslie B. Vosshall 
fruitless Splicing Specifies Male Courtship Behavior in Drosophila
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages e3 (June 2017)
A Leptin Analog Locally Produced in the Brain Acts via a Conserved Neural Circuit to Modulate Obesity-Linked Behaviors in Drosophila  Jennifer Beshel,
Masayuki Koganezawa, Ken-ichi Kimura, Daisuke Yamamoto  Current Biology 
Volume 96, Issue 4, Pages e4 (November 2017)
Volume 23, Issue 3, Pages (February 2013)
Volume 28, Issue 9, Pages e3 (May 2018)
Neuronal Control of Drosophila Courtship Song
Volume 23, Issue 13, Pages (July 2013)
Volume 22, Issue 13, Pages (July 2012)
Volume 75, Issue 4, Pages (August 2012)
James J.L. Hodge, Praseeda Mullasseril, Leslie C. Griffith  Neuron 
Volume 24, Issue 6, Pages (August 2018)
Volume 19, Issue 5, Pages (March 2009)
Dopaminergic Circuitry Underlying Mating Drive
Troy R. Shirangi, Allan M. Wong, James W. Truman, David L. Stern 
Neural Circuitry that Governs Drosophila Male Courtship Behavior
Abhishek Chatterjee, Shintaro Tanoue, Jerry H. Houl, Paul E. Hardin 
Watching the Fly Brain Learn
A GABAergic Feedback Shapes Dopaminergic Input on the Drosophila Mushroom Body to Promote Appetitive Long-Term Memory  Alice Pavlowsky, Johann Schor,
Mario R. Pagani, Kimihiko Oishi, Bruce D. Gelb, Yi Zhong  Cell 
Drosophila CRYPTOCHROME Is a Circadian Transcriptional Repressor
Female Contact Activates Male-Specific Interneurons that Trigger Stereotypic Courtship Behavior in Drosophila  Soh Kohatsu, Masayuki Koganezawa, Daisuke.
Volume 25, Issue 20, Pages (October 2015)
A Wolbachia-Sensitive Communication between Male and Female Pupae Controls Gamete Compatibility in Drosophila  Stéphanie M. Pontier, François Schweisguth 
Volume 132, Issue 6, Pages (March 2008)
insomniac and Cullin-3 Regulate Sleep and Wakefulness in Drosophila
Sex and the Single Splice
Evolution of yellow Gene Regulation and Pigmentation in Drosophila
Volume 25, Issue 11, Pages (June 2015)
Let-7-Complex MicroRNAs Regulate the Temporal Identity of Drosophila Mushroom Body Neurons via chinmo  Yen-Chi Wu, Ching-Huan Chen, Adam Mercer, Nicholas S.
Clock and cycle Limit Starvation-Induced Sleep Loss in Drosophila
Samuel James Walker, Verónica María Corrales-Carvajal, Carlos Ribeiro 
Extinction Antagonizes Olfactory Memory at the Subcellular Level
Pallavi Lamba, Diana Bilodeau-Wentworth, Patrick Emery, Yong Zhang 
Volume 165, Issue 4, Pages (May 2016)
Volume 22, Issue 19, Pages (October 2012)
Martin Häsemeyer, Nilay Yapici, Ulrike Heberlein, Barry J. Dickson 
Volume 25, Issue 22, Pages (November 2015)
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
Susan J Broughton, Toshihiro Kitamoto, Ralph J Greenspan 
Volume 24, Issue 7, Pages (March 2014)
Piezo-like Gene Regulates Locomotion in Drosophila Larvae
Volume 23, Issue 6, Pages (May 2018)
Allison L. Blum, Wanhe Li, Mike Cressy, Josh Dubnau  Current Biology 
Volume 8, Issue 21, Pages (October 1998)
Volume 83, Issue 1, Pages (July 2014)
Irreplaceability of Neuronal Ensembles after Memory Allocation
Masayuki Koganezawa, Ken-ichi Kimura, Daisuke Yamamoto  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Genetic Identification and Separation of Innate and Experience-Dependent Courtship Behaviors in Drosophila  Yufeng Pan, Bruce S. Baker  Cell  Volume 156, Issue 1, Pages 236-248 (January 2014) DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 In the Absence of fruM Function D. melanogaster Males Acquire the Ability to Court as a Consequence of Being Group-Housed with Other Flies (A) Courtship indices of males that had been isolated since eclosion prior to a 10 min single-pair courtship test. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. (B) Chaining indices of groups of eight males that had been housed together for 4 days. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. (C) Chaining indices of groups of eight or five fruLexA/fru4–40 males and courtship indices between 2 fruLexA/fru4–40 males after grouping for 3 hr, 1 day, 2 days, 3 days, and 4 days. (D) Courtship indices of fruLexA/fru4–40 males to wild-type females after grouping (in groups of one male with seven females, one male with four females, or one male with one female) from 3 hr to 4 days. n = 8–12 for chaining behavior; n = 12–24 for others. n refers to number of pairs or groups, housing conditions are indicated in the top of each panel (apply to all figures). Error bars represent SEM. See also Figure S1, Movies S1, and S2. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 fruM and dsxM Overlapping Neurons Are Required for Both Wild-Type Courtship and fruM-Independent Courtship (A) A schematic of genetic strategy to label and manipulate fruM and dsx overlapping neurons. (B and C) Intersectional expression of fruLexA and dsxGAL4(Δ2) in wild-type (B) and fruM null (C) backgrounds (genotypes: LexAop2-FlpL/UAS > stop > myrGFP; dsxGAL4(Δ2) fruLexA/+ and LexAop2-FlpL/UAS > stop > myrGFP; dsxGAL4(Δ2) fruLexA/fru4–40, respectively). Arrow indicates no midline crossing by gustatory receptor neurons in fruM null background. Scale bars represent 100 μm. (D) Courtship indices (upper) and copulation indices (lower) by males that have fruM function. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. n = 24 for each. (E–G) Chaining indices by fruM null males after grouping from 3 hr to 6 days. n = 12 for each. Not significant (N.S.) by comparing chaining indices of day 6. Detailed genotypes are described in text. Error bars represent SEM. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 dsxM Is Necessary and Sufficient in Both XX and XY Flies for fruM-Independent Courtship Behavior (A) Expression of sex-specific products of fruM and dsx in wild-type males and females, as well as in fruM null males and dsx-masculinized females. (B) Chaining indices of groups of eight XX;;dsx683–7058/dsxS flies after grouping from 3 hr to 6 days. (C) Courtship indices of individual XX;;dsx683–7058/dsxS flies to seven wild-type females or females of other species (D. yakuba or D. mojavensis). Not significant (N.S.) by comparing courtship indices of day 6. (D) Chaining indices of groups of eight XX flies after 6-day grouping. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, compared with 0, one-sample t test. (E and F) Chaining indices of groups of eight XY flies after 6-day grouping. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. n = 8–12 for each. Genotypes indicated below. Error bars indicate SEM. See also Movie S3. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Grouping-Induced Courtship Behavior in fruM Null Males Is Long-Lasting (A) Courtship indices of fruLexA/fru4–40 males, after being isolated (triangle) or grouped with wild-type females (square), to fresh wild-type females. X axis indicates number of days being isolated (triangle) or grouped (square) prior to testing. n = 12 for each. (B) Courtship indices of fruLexA/fru4–40 males, after being grouped with females for 8 days and then isolated from grouping for up to 7 days, to fresh females. X axis indicates number of days being isolated following the 8 day grouping and prior to testing. n = 12 for each. (C) Detail steps of courtship displayed by fruLexA/fru4–40 males after group-housing, compared with courtship by single-housed wild-type males. Arrows indicate tapping, wing extension, proboscis extension, abdomen bending, and attempted copulation, respectively. n = 24 for each. (D) Latency of following and wing extension by fruM null males and wild-type males. (E) Percentage of time displaying following and wing extension by fruM null males and wild-type males. N.S., not significant; ∗∗∗p < 0.001, two-sample t test. Error bars represent SEM. See also Figure S2, Movie S4, and S5. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Flexibility of the Grouping-Induced Courtship Behavior in fruM Null Males All males were tested either between two males of the same genotype (blue), toward D. melanogaster females (red), D. yakuba females (yellow), or D. mojavensis females (cyan). (A–E) Courtship indices of fruLexA/fru4–40 males that had been isolated (A), or grouped with males of the same genotype (B), D. melanogaster females (C), D. yakuba females (D), or D. mojavensis females (E) for 8 days prior to testing. ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01 and ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. (F–I) Courtship indices of wild-type males that had been isolated (F), or grouped with males of the same genotype (G), D. yakuba females (H), or D. mojavensis females (I) for 8 days prior to testing. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. n = 12–24 for each. Error bars represent SEM. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 Sensory Basis of the Grouping-Induced Courtship Behavior in fruM Null Males (A–E) Courtship indices by fruLexA/fru4–40 males with male (gray) or female (white) targets. Manipulations of visual inputs during group-housing and/or courtship testing are described (highlighted by underlines). ∗∗∗p < 0.001, two-sample t test. (F) Courtship indices by fruLexA/fru4–40 males with Orco, ppk23, or ppk25 mutations. Detailed genotypes are described in text. n = 12–24 for each. Error bars represent SEM. Please see Figure S2. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 7 Alternative Modes of Specifying Male Courtship in Drosophila As dsx is expressed in both nonneuronal somatic cells as well as neurons, we distinguish its role in the nervous system for male courtship from nonneuronal somatic development. In contrast, fruM is expressed exclusively in neurons. Left: FRUM and DSXM jointly specify wild-type courtship that is conspecific female directed. Right: in the absence of FRUM, social experience and DSXM jointly specify the experience-dependent courtship that is more flexible. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure S1 Group-Housing Induces Courtship Behavior in Multiple fruM Null Combinations, Related to Figure 1 (A) Organization of the fru gene. P1-P4 indicate alternative promoters. Only the P1 transcripts are sex-specifically spliced to yield male-specific FRUM proteins. S is the sex-specifically spliced exon. A–C indicate alternative 3′ exons. fruLexA is an insertion into the male-specific exon (gray). fru4-40, frusat15 and fruAJ96u3 are deficiency lines where the horizontal black bars indicate sequences deleted. (B–D) Chaining indices of groups of 8 males (square) and courtship indices of 1 male to 7 females (triangle) after grouping from 3 hr to 4 days. n = 8 for male chaining, and n = 12 for male-female courtship. Error bars indicate SEM. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure S2 The Acquisition of Courtship Behavior by fruM Null Males Requires Olfactory Receptor Neurons but Not Mushroom Bodies, Related to Figures 4 and 6 (A and B) Expression pattern of two GAL4 lines (19B03 and 76D11) that express broadly in mushroom bodies. (C) Courtship indices toward females by fruM null males that express shits1 in GAL4-labeled cells. These males were group housed at permissive temperature (23°C) and tested at either permissive (25°C, blue) or restrictive (30°C, red) temperatures. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, two-sample t test. (D) Courtship indices of fruM null males that express kir2.1::GFP in GAL4-labeled cells toward females. To induce and maintain kir2.1::GFP expression during adult, flies were heat shocked at 29°C for 1 day and then continuously raised at 25°C. ∗∗∗p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA. n = 12∼24 for each. Error bars indicate SEM. (E) Expression of kir2.1::GFP in a male brain dissected immediately after courtship test. No antibody was applied. Scale bars: 100μm. Cell 2014 156, 236-248DOI: (10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.041) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions