Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Circadian Timekeeping System of Drosophila Paul E. Hardin Current Biology Volume 15, Issue 17, Pages R714-R722 (September 2005) DOI: /j.cub
Advertisements

Sea turtles Current Biology
Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
Matthew Collett, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Human sleep and cortical reactivity are influenced by lunar phase
Cell Adhesion: Sizing Up a Sticky Situation
Convergent Evolution: The Genetics of Queen Number in Ants
Volume 14, Issue 6, Pages R221-R224 (March 2004)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Biomechanics: An Army Marching with Its Stomach
Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Mandyam V. Srinivasan 
Shape Control: Cell Growth Hits the Mechanical Buffers
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Episodic Memory: Rats Master Multiple Memories
Oogenesis: Matrix Revolutions
How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Navigation: Whence Our Sense of Direction?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Volume 26, Issue 24, Pages R1266-R1267 (December 2016)
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e2 (September 2017)
Infant cognition Current Biology
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
Visual Perception: Lightness in a High-Dynamic-Range World
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Shape Control: Cell Growth Hits the Mechanical Buffers
Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 25, Issue 13, Pages (June 2015)
Human colour perception changes between seasons
The many colours of ‘the dress’
Purple Tomatoes: Longer Lasting, Less Disease, and Better for You
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Sea turtles Current Biology
Nikita Chernetsov, Dmitry Kishkinev, Henrik Mouritsen  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 7, Pages (April 2008)
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Thomas S Collett, Paul Graham  Current Biology 
Robert A. Harris, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic information in macaque inferior temporal cortex  Kasper Vinken, Rufin Vogels  Current Biology 
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Adrienn G. Varga, Roy E. Ritzmann  Current Biology 
Active Vision: Adapting How to Look
Volume 23, Issue 24, Pages R1083-R1085 (December 2013)
Neuronal Plasticity: How Do Neurons Know What To Do?
Ian C. Fiebelkorn, Yuri B. Saalmann, Sabine Kastner  Current Biology 
Brain Stimulation: Feeling the Buzz
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Volume 19, Issue 20, Pages R922-R923 (November 2009)
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Drosophila Connectomics: Mapping the Larval Eye’s Mind
Eurasian reed warblers compensate for virtual magnetic displacement
Calibration of vector navigation in desert ants
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation Paul Graham, Ken Cheng  Current Biology  Volume 19, Issue 20, Pages R935-R937 (November 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.015 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Headings of ants in response to artificial panoramic skyline. (A) The nest (n) locale with an ad lib feeder (f) 5 m from the nest entrance. (B) The panoramic view from the feeder location. (C) 1-m radius panoramic arena with variable-height walls. (D,E) Panoramic views from the centre of the arena with the artificial skyline. The retinal height of the natural skyline was matched every 15° and linear interpolation used in between. (D) Skyline cues are aligned with habitual Feeder–Nest direction. (E) Test arena rotated clockwise by 150°. (F–H) Circular histograms of ants' headings after 15 cm in the test arena. Direction and length of the red arrow represent the direction and length of the mean vector for each distribution. All three distributions of headings (F–H) are significantly non-uniform (Rayleigh Test, p ≪ 0.005). Black arrows with ranges represent the mean compass bearing (with 95% confidence interval) of zero-vector ants from Nest 1 (F, G; n = 25) and Nest 2 (H; n = 26) returning home from the feeder at the training ground. The headings of Nest 1 and Nest 2 zero-vector ants are significantly different (Watson-Williams Test, p ≪0.005). Grey arrows represent predicted heading for ants using skyline cues. This heading is derived from the mean heading of zero-vector ants at the training ground (black arrows) and the orientation of the artificial arena. In all three cases (F–H) there is no significant difference (Watson-Williams Test) between the observed headings of zero-vector ants in the test arena and at the feeder, relative to the skyline profile. There is a significant difference (Watson-Williams Test, p ≪ 0.005) in the headings of Nest 1 and Nest 2 ants in the rotated arena (G,H). Current Biology 2009 19, R935-R937DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.08.015) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions