Collective Behavior: Social Digging in Drosophila Larvae

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Circadian Timekeeping System of Drosophila Paul E. Hardin Current Biology Volume 15, Issue 17, Pages R714-R722 (September 2005) DOI: /j.cub
Advertisements

Sea turtles Current Biology
Nutrition: Rejection Is the Fly’s Protection
Behavioral Neuroscience: Crawling Is a No-Brainer for Fruit Fly Larvae
Calibrating color vision
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
The nature of Drosophila melanogaster
Brain Plasticity: When the Feet and Mouth Replace the Hand
Jean-Paul Noel, Mark Wallace, Randolph Blake  Current Biology 
Mitosis: New Roles for Myosin-X and Actin at the Spindle
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Obesity: Cognitive Impairment and the Failure to ‘Eat Right’
Biomechanics: An Army Marching with Its Stomach
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Theta Rhythm: Temporal Glue for Episodic Memory
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Intracellular Transport: Kinesins Working Together
Navigation: Whence Our Sense of Direction?
Lymphoid tissue inducer cells
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
Whisking Current Biology
Meiosis: Organizing Microtubule Organizers
Behavioural Genetics: Evolutionary Fingerprint of the ‘Invisible Hand’
Infant cognition Current Biology
Volume 24, Issue 5, Pages R204-R206 (March 2014)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Evolutionary Genetics: Reuse, Recycle, Converge
Visual System: Prostriata — A Visual Area Off the Beaten Path
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Volume 16, Issue 17, Pages R682-R684 (September 2006)
Cooperative Behavior Emerges among Drosophila Larvae
Visual Cortex: The Eccentric Area Prostriata in the Human Brain
Visual Attention: Size Matters
The real ‘domains’ of life
Face Perception: Broken into Parts
Volume 18, Issue 11, Pages R453-R455 (June 2008)
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Honeybee Communication: A Signal for Danger
An Embodied View of Octopus Neurobiology
Evolution: Mirror, Mirror in the Pond
Sea turtles Current Biology
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Imaginal discs Current Biology
Figs and fig wasps Current Biology
Animal Behavior: Timing in the Wild
Gene Regulation: Activation through Space
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Tool Use: Crows Craft the Right Tool for the Job
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages R784-R785 (September 2012)
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Federica Amici, Filippo Aureli, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Neuronal Plasticity: How Do Neurons Know What To Do?
Burying beetles Current Biology
Social Coordination: Patience Is a Virtue for Vervet Monkeys
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Planar Cell Polarity: A Bridge Too Far?
Volume 28, Issue 2, Pages R58-R60 (January 2018)
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages R350-R351 (April 2018)
Sensory Neurophysiology: Motion Vision during Motor Action
Animal Communication: Origins of Sequential Structure in Birdsong
Crocodile egg sounds signal hatching time
Multi-Cue Integration: How Female Mosquitoes Locate a Human Host
Vision: Attending the Invisible
The mushroom body Current Biology
Human Evolution: Genomic Gifts from Archaic Hominins
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Presentation transcript:

Collective Behavior: Social Digging in Drosophila Larvae Matthieu Louis, Gonzalo de Polavieja  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages R1010-R1012 (September 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.023 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Coordinating collective digging behavior between larvae. (A) Picture of a cylindrical culture vial in the lab. The closed-up view displays the grouping of larvae along the plastic wall of the vial. Larvae dig with their head down; the mouth hooks at the anterior of the head are visible as black spots at the lower extremities of individual larvae. (B) Temporal sequence of social digging. (Bi) Shortly after their emergence from the egg, young larvae wander on the food media (orange color). Through a search process directed by pheromone molecules [11], larvae aggregate on the surface. (Bii) Groups of larvae dig into the food media. The posterior spiracles — extremities of the breathing tubes that traverse the whole body — remain in contact with the surface. Using the spiracles as “snorkeling” devices, larvae feed through regular contractions of their mouth hooks [4,9]. Production of digestive enzymes softens the food media (yellow color), thereby facilitating digging and feeding. (Biii) Occasionally a cluster of larvae breaks apart. It is possible that the lack of oxygen within the cluster is a factor that triggers the dissociation of the group. (C) To create and maintain an organized cluster, larvae must synchronize the phase of their periodic mouth-hook contractions. Dombrovski et al. [4] found that coordinated digging behavior requires visual cues; mechanosensory cues associated with the movement of neighboring larvae and/or the meniscus of the substrate might play a coordinating role as well. (D) We suggest that the behavior of digging larvae can be approximated as harmonic oscillators. The Kuramoto model describes the behavior of a large set of nearly identical and weakly coupled oscillators. The phase of a given oscillator ϕi changes because of its intrinsic frequency ωi and its coupling to the other oscillators. When the coupling constant C is sufficiently strong, the group of oscillators can synchronize [3]. Current Biology 2017 27, R1010-R1012DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.023) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions