False-belief understanding in infants

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
False Belief Understanding in Infants -Dr. Renee Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott, Zijing He - Kuldeep Yadav (10358)
Advertisements

Presenter: Mai Hinton Mentors: Lindsey Powell & Kate Hobbs February 2011.
Origins of the specialization for letters and numbers in ventral occipitotemporal cortex Thomas Hannagan, Amir Amedi, Laurent Cohen, Ghislaine Dehaene-Lambertz,
The magic of the sugar code Hans-Joachim Gabius Trends in Biochemical Sciences Volume 40, Issue 7, (July 2015) DOI: /j.tibs Copyright.
Psych 56L/ Ling 51: Acquisition of Language Lecture 16 Language & Cognition.
Speciation by symbiosis Robert M. Brucker, Seth R. Bordenstein Trends in Ecology & Evolution Volume 27, Issue 8, Pages (August 2012) DOI: /j.tree
Multifaceted roles for lipids in viral infection Nicholas S. Heaton, Glenn Randall Trends in Microbiology Volume 19, Issue 7, Pages (July 2011)
Advances in shaking technologies Wolf Klöckner, Jochen Büchs Trends in Biotechnology Volume 30, Issue 6, Pages (June 2012) DOI: /j.tibtech
How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Luiz Pessoa Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 13, Issue 4, Pages (April 2009) DOI: /j.tics
Does the contraceptive pill alter mate choice in humans? Alexandra Alvergne, Virpi Lummaa Trends in Ecology & Evolution Volume 25, Issue 3, Pages
Two views of brain function Marcus E. Raichle Trends in Cognitive Sciences Volume 14, Issue 4, Pages (April 2010) DOI: /j.tics
The Microbiome and Cancer: Is the ‘Oncobiome’ Mirage Real? Ryan M. Thomas, Christian Jobin Trends in Cancer Volume 1, Issue 1, Pages (September 2015)
Healthy clocks, healthy body, healthy mind Akhilesh B. Reddy, John S. O’Neill Trends in Cell Biology Volume 20, Issue 1, Pages (January 2010) DOI:
Primary microcephaly: do all roads lead to Rome? Gemma K. Thornton, C. Geoffrey Woods Trends in Genetics Volume 25, Issue 11, Pages (November 2009)
Captain Leslie Russell Blake and Aberdeen University’s penguin egg Martyn L. Gorman Current Biology Volume 15, Issue 11, Pages R402-R405 (June 2005) DOI:
Show and Tell: imitation by an 10-minute-old
“We propose that the present results suggest that
Comparative thanatology
Cognitive development
Ganesh Vigneswaran, Roland Philipp, Roger N. Lemon, Alexander Kraskov 
Identifying and validating biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease
21st century diabetes care: a marriage between humans and technology
The Developing Social Brain: Implications for Education
The Developing Social Brain: Implications for Education
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
(Radio)active Neurogenesis in the Human Hippocampus
Communication in Drug Development: “Translating” Scientific Discovery
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Identifying and validating biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease
Expanding perception through the disordered brain
Advances in fMRI Real-Time Neurofeedback
Infant cognition Current Biology
The Architecture of Restriction Enzymes
Breastfeeding in the 21st century
Doing science making art
Volume 39, Issue 6, Pages (September 2010)
Models of visual word recognition
HIV myths should not be resuscitated
I Know What You Are Doing
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Reputation management in the age of the world-wide web
Bonobos Prefer Individuals that Hinder Others over Those that Help
Working memory without consciousness
Making a New Turn In Matrix Metalloprotease Inhibition
Advances in fMRI Real-Time Neurofeedback
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Unjustified regulation prevents use of GMO technology for public good
The Evolutionary Origins of Human Patience: Temporal Preferences in Chimpanzees, Bonobos, and Human Adults  Alexandra G. Rosati, Jeffrey R. Stevens, Brian.
The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy
Septins: Cellular and Functional Barriers of Neuronal Activity
Self and Other in the Human Motor System
Volume 17, Issue 1, Pages (January 2007)
Public Versus Personal Information for Mate Copying in an Invertebrate
“We propose that the present results suggest that
Volume 21, Issue 9, Pages (September 2014)
Volume 19, Issue 15, Pages (August 2009)
Response to Visual Threat Following Damage to the Pulvinar
Looking for cognition in the structure within the noise
Visual Perception: Understanding Visual Cues to Depth
Male Fish Deceive Competitors about Mating Preferences
Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs
Comparative thanatology
Jonathan Redshaw, Thomas Suddendorf  Current Biology 
Will Travel for Food: Spatial Discounting in Two New World Monkeys
Volume 21, Issue 9, Pages (September 2014)
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Making a Division Apparatus on Mitochondria
A disease of the osteoblast
Presentation transcript:

False-belief understanding in infants Renée Baillargeon, Rose M. Scott, Zijing He  Trends in Cognitive Sciences  Volume 14, Issue 3, Pages 110-118 (March 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Can 15-month-olds attribute to an agent a false belief about an object's location? In the false-belief-green and false-belief-yellow conditions of Onishi and Baillargeon [16], the infants first received three familiarization trials. In trial 1, a toy stood between a yellow and a green box; a female agent entered the apparatus, played with the toy briefly, hid it inside the green box, and then paused, with her hand inside the green box, until the trial ended. In trials 2 and 3, the agent reached inside the green box, as though to grasp her toy, and then paused. In the belief-induction trial, the toy either moved from the green to the yellow box in the agent's absence (false-belief-green condition) or moved to the yellow box in the agent's presence but then returned to the green box after she left (false-belief-yellow condition). In the test trial, the agent returned, reached inside either the yellow box (yellow-box event) or the green box (green-box event), and then paused. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010 14, 110-118DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Can 14.5-month-olds attribute to an agent a false perception of an object? In the doll and skunk conditions of Song and Baillargeon [21], the infants first received four familiarization trials. In each trial, a female agent sat behind a doll with blue pigtails and a stuffed skunk with a pink bow. An experimenter's gloved hands placed the toys on placemats in trials 1 and 2 and inside shallow containers in trials 3 and 4. The agent always reached for either the doll (doll condition) or the skunk (skunk condition), suggesting that she preferred it to the other toy. In the box-orientation trial, the agent was absent; two large boxes with lids rested on the apparatus floor and the experimenter rotated each lid in turn, demonstrating that the right box's lid had a tuft of blue hair (similar to the doll's) attached to it. At the start of the test trial, the agent was again absent; the experimenter hid the doll in the plain box and the skunk in the hair box. The agent then returned, reached for either the plain box (plain-box event) or the hair box (hair-box event), and paused. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010 14, 110-118DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Can 18-month-olds attribute to an agent a false belief about an object's identity? In the false-belief experiment of Scott and Baillargeon [4], the infants received four familiarization trials involving two toy penguins that were identical except that one could come apart (2-piece penguin) and one could not (1-piece penguin). As a female agent watched, an experimenter's gloved hands placed the 1-piece penguin and the two pieces of the disassembled 2-piece penguin on platforms in trials 1 and 2 and in shallow containers in trials 3 and 4. The agent then placed a key in the bottom piece of the 2-piece penguin, stacked the two pieces, and paused. During the test trials, while the agent was absent, the experimenter assembled the 2-piece penguin, covered it with a transparent cover, and then covered the 1-piece penguin with an opaque cover. The agent then entered the apparatus with her key, reached for either the transparent cover (transparent-cover event) or the opaque cover (opaque-cover event), and paused. Order of presentation of the two test events was counterbalanced. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010 14, 110-118DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Do 18-month-olds hold different expectations for an agent who is ignorant as opposed to mistaken about an object's location? In the ignorance experiment of Scott and Baillargeon [4], the infants received the same familiarization trials as in the false-belief experiment (Figure 3). The test trials were also similar to those in the false-belief experiment, except that the two covers were both opaque, so that the agent had no basis for determining which cover hid which penguin and was therefore ignorant about the location of the 2-piece penguin. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2010 14, 110-118DOI: (10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions