The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Risk-Responsive Orbitofrontal Neurons Track Acquired Salience
Advertisements

Takafumi Minamimoto, Richard C. Saunders, Barry J. Richmond  Neuron 
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Grid Cells for Conceptual Spaces?
David L. Barack, Steve W.C. Chang, Michael L. Platt  Neuron 
Volume 73, Issue 3, Pages (February 2012)
To Detect and Correct: Norm Violations and Their Enforcement
Effective Connectivity between Hippocampus and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Controls Preferential Choices from Memory  Sebastian Gluth, Tobias Sommer,
René Quilodran, Marie Rothé, Emmanuel Procyk  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Frontal Cortex and the Discovery of Abstract Action Rules
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Sang Wan Lee, Shinsuke Shimojo, John P. O’Doherty  Neuron 
Decision Making: From Neuroscience to Psychiatry
Martin O'Neill, Wolfram Schultz  Neuron 
Visual Attention: Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Sheng Li, Stephen D. Mayhew, Zoe Kourtzi  Neuron 
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Neural Correlates of Knowledge: Stable Representation of Stimulus Associations across Variations in Behavioral Performance  Adam Messinger, Larry R. Squire,
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Probabilistic Population Codes for Bayesian Decision Making
Sensitivity to Complex Statistical Regularities in Rat Auditory Cortex
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Volume 19, Issue 18, Pages (September 2009)
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Decision Making: From Neuroscience to Psychiatry
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Decoding Cognitive Processes from Neural Ensembles
Volume 73, Issue 3, Pages (February 2012)
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e6 (April 2017)
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Volume 74, Issue 4, Pages (May 2012)
Prefrontal Cortex Activity Related to Abstract Response Strategies
Caleb E. Strait, Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden  Neuron 
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Memory: Enduring Traces of Perceptual and Reflective Attention
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Why Seeing Is Believing: Merging Auditory and Visual Worlds
Broca's Area and the Hierarchical Organization of Human Behavior
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Sarah R. Heilbronner, Michael L. Platt  Neuron 
Social Attention and the Brain
Caroline A. Montojo, Susan M. Courtney  Neuron 
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Richard A. Andersen, He Cui  Neuron 
Matthew R. Roesch, Adam R. Taylor, Geoffrey Schoenbaum  Neuron 
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff in Olfaction
Presentation transcript:

The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity Celeste Kidd, Benjamin Y. Hayden  Neuron  Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages 449-460 (November 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Curiosity and Information-Seeking (A) Data from Kinney and Kagan (1976). Attention to auditory stimuli shows an inverted U-shaped pattern, with infants making the most fixations to auditory stimuli estimated to be moderately discrepant from the auditory stimuli for which infants already possessed mental representations. (B) Data from Kang et al. (2009). Subjects were most curious about the answers to trivia questions for which they were moderately confident about their answers. This pattern suggests that subjects exhibited the greatest curiosity for information that was partially—but not fully—encoded. Neuron 2015 88, 449-460DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Bandit Task (A) In a four-arm restless bandit task, subjects choose one of four targets in each trial. (B) The value associated with each option changes stoichastically in value (uncued) in each trial. Consequently, when the subject has identified the best target, there is a benefit to occasionally interspersing trials where an alternative is chosen (exploration) into the more common pattern of choosing the known best option (exploitation). For example, the subject may choose option A (red color) for several trials but would not know that blue (B) will soon overtake A in value without occasionally exploring other options. (C) In this task, neurons in the posterior cingulate cortex show higher tonic firing in explore trials than in exploit trials. ITI, inter-trial interval. Neuron 2015 88, 449-460DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Curiosity Tradeoff Task (A) In the curiosity tradeoff task, subjects choose between two gambles that vary in informativeness (cyan versus magenta) and gamble stakes (the size of the white inset bar). In each trial, two gambles appear in sequence on a computer screen (indicated by a black rectangle). When both options appear, subjects shift their gaze to one to select it. Then, after 2.25 s, they receive a juice reward. Following choice of the informative option, they receive a cue telling them whether they get the reward (50% chance). Following choice of the uninformative option, subjects get not valid information. (B) Two subjects both showed a preference for informative options (indicated by a left shift of the psychometric curve) over uninformative ones despite the fact that this information provided no strategic benefit. (C) In this task, OFC does not integrate value because of information (vertical axis) with value because of reward size (horizontal axis). Neuron 2015 88, 449-460DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Curiosity and Attention (A) Example display from Kidd et al. (2012). Each display featured three unique boxes hiding three unique objects that revealed themselves one at a time according to one of 32 sequences of varying complexity. The sequence continued until the infant looked away for 1 s. (B) Infant look-away data plotted by complexity (information content) as estimated by an ideal observer model over the transitional probabilities. The U-shaped pattern indicates that infants were least likely to look away at events with intermediate information content. The infants’ probability of looking away was greatest to events of either very low information content (highly predictable) or very high information content (highly surprising), consistent with an attentional strategy that aims to maintain intermediate rates of information absorption. Neuron 2015 88, 449-460DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Curiosity and Learning Experimental stimuli from Schulz and Bonawitz (2007). When both levers were pressed simultaneously, two puppets (a straw pom-pom and a chick) emerged from the center of the box. In this confounded case, the evidence was not informative about which of the two levers caused each puppet to rise. Under the unconfounded conditions, one lever was pressed at a time, making it clear which lever caused each puppet to rise. During a free play period following the toy’s demonstration, children played more with the toy when the demonstrated evidence was confounded. Neuron 2015 88, 449-460DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions