Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Sea turtles Current Biology
Advertisements

Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 11, Pages (June 2012)
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Volume 23, Issue 19, Pages (October 2013)
Animal Cognition: How Archer Fish Learn to Down Rapidly Moving Targets
Volume 23, Issue 4, Pages (February 2013)
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Animal Vision: Rats Watch the Sky
Jacob R. Goheen, Todd M. Palmer  Current Biology 
Coldness Triggers Northward Flight in Remigrant Monarch Butterflies
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages (June 2014)
Volume 26, Issue 24, Pages R1266-R1267 (December 2016)
Social Feedback and the Emergence of Leaders and Followers
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e2 (September 2017)
The Role of the Magnetite-Based Receptors in the Beak in Pigeon Homing
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Polarized Light Helps Monarch Butterflies Navigate
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Depth Affects Where We Look
Climate-Mediated Movement of an Avian Hybrid Zone
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation
Context-dependent lateralized feeding strategies in blue whales
Longitude Perception and Bicoordinate Magnetic Maps in Sea Turtles
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
A Magnetic Map Leads Juvenile European Eels to the Gulf Stream
Volume 19, Issue 12, Pages (June 2009)
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Marianne Elias, Colin Fontaine, F.J. Frank van Veen  Current Biology 
Box Jellyfish Use Terrestrial Visual Cues for Navigation
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Susanne Åkesson, Jens Morin, Rachel Muheim, Ulf Ottosson 
Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex
Sea turtles Current Biology
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Nikita Chernetsov, Dmitry Kishkinev, Henrik Mouritsen  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 7, Pages (April 2008)
Jake E. Bicknell, Matthew J. Struebig, David P. Edwards, Zoe G. Davies 
Restorative Justice in Children
Evidence that Magnetic Navigation and Geomagnetic Imprinting Shape Spatial Genetic Variation in Sea Turtles  J. Roger Brothers, Kenneth J. Lohmann  Current.
Asif A. Ghazanfar, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, W. Tecumseh Fitch 
Communal Nutrition in Ants
Shade coffee farms promote genetic diversity of native trees
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Volume 22, Issue 1, Pages (January 2012)
Volume 18, Issue 17, Pages R728-R729 (September 2008)
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (March 2008)
Path Integration: Combining Optic Flow with Compass Orientation
Zuzana Burivalova, Çağan Hakkı Şekercioğlu, Lian Pin Koh 
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Ian C. Fiebelkorn, Yuri B. Saalmann, Sabine Kastner  Current Biology 
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Rapid Evolution of the Cerebellum in Humans and Other Great Apes
Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages (July 2015)
Volume 15, Issue 9, Pages (May 2005)
The Geography of Ecological Niche Evolution in Mammals
Synergistic Effects of Marine Reserves and Harvest Controls on the Abundance and Catch Dynamics of a Coral Reef Fishery  Jess K. Hopf, Geoffrey P. Jones,
Eurasian reed warblers compensate for virtual magnetic displacement
Biodiversity and Ecosystem Functioning: The Mystery of the Deep Sea
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Fig. 3 Global and basin-averaged sampling error compared with reconstructed temperature change. Global and basin-averaged sampling error compared with.
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages 446-450 (February 2014) An Inherited Magnetic Map Guides Ocean Navigation in Juvenile Pacific Salmon  Nathan F. Putman, Michelle M. Scanlan, Eric J. Billman, Joseph P. O’Neil, Ryan B. Couture, Thomas P. Quinn, Kenneth J. Lohmann, David L.G. Noakes  Current Biology  Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages 446-450 (February 2014) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.017 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Orientation of Chinook Salmon to Magnetic Fields in the North Pacific Circular histograms showing the orientation of juvenile Chinook salmon when presented with magnetic fields that exist in the northeastern Pacific Ocean. Magnetic north = 0°; length of gray bars indicates the number of fish that were oriented within each 15° range of directions. The circle edge is scaled to 21 individuals. The black triangle indicates the mean heading of each group; the dotted line on the outside of the circle represents the 95% confidence interval of the mean (shown for distributions that were significantly oriented). See Figure 3 for locations. (A) Fish tested in a magnetic field that exists at the northern periphery of their oceanic range were oriented to the south-southwest (mean heading = 216°, Rayleigh r = 0.135, Rayleigh p = 0.014, n = 233). (B) Orientation of fish tested in the ambient magnetic field at the testing location could not be distinguished from random (Rayleigh r = 0.048, Rayleigh p = 0.582, n = 240). (C) Fish tested in a magnetic field that exists at the southern periphery of their oceanic range were oriented to the north-northeast (mean heading = 17°, Rayleigh r = 0.163. Rayleigh p = 0.002, n = 234). Current Biology 2014 24, 446-450DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.017) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Orientation of Chinook Salmon to Conflicting Magnetic Cues Circular histograms showing the orientation of juvenile Chinook salmon when presented with magnetic fields with contradictory magnetic information (conventions as in Figure 1). In both treatments, fish orientation could not be distinguished from random, indicating that orientation responses are not attributable to only one magnetic parameter. (A) Orientation to the northern intensity and southern inclination angle (Rayleigh r = 0.024, Rayleigh p = 0.867, n = 240). (B) Orientation to the southern intensity and northern inclination angle (Rayleigh r = 0.093, Rayleigh p = 0.132, n = 232). Current Biology 2014 24, 446-450DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.017) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Locations in the Northeast Pacific Ocean Used in Simulated Magnetic Displacements Maps show the test location (white circle) and the isolines of magnetic intensity and inclination angles presented during experiments, relative to the oceanic range of Chinook salmon. The actual range of the Chinook used in our study is unknown, but it is probably more widespread than the area where Chinook from Oregon are caught by commercial and sport fisherman (dark blue shading) [39] and less extensive than the full range of the species (light blue shading) [40]. Solid isodynamics indicate the northern and southern intensity used in experiments (555.5 μT and 444.6 μT, respectively). Dashed isoclinics indicate the northern and southern inclination angle (73.3° and 56.7°, respectively). Circles with crosses show the intersection of these two coordinates in 2014 (A), 1957 (B), and 1900 (C). Though intensity has drifted considerably (e.g., from 1940 to present, the northern intensity isodynamic is north of the test site, whereas prior to 1940, this value existed both to the north and south of the test site), the pairings of magnetic inclination angle and intensity that we presented to fish have existed in the vicinity of the latitudinal extremes of the oceanic range of Chinook for more than a century. Thus, observed orientation responses to the intensity and inclination angle pairings (Figure 1) appear to be adaptive given recent rates of field drift. Map projection is cylindrical equidistant; magnetic data are based on IGRF-11 [41]. Current Biology 2014 24, 446-450DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.017) Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions