Marine Battesti, Celine Moreno, Dominique Joly, Frederic Mery 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Circadian Timekeeping System of Drosophila Paul E. Hardin Current Biology Volume 15, Issue 17, Pages R714-R722 (September 2005) DOI: /j.cub
Advertisements

Aimee S. Dunlap, Matthew E. Nielsen, Anna Dornhaus, Daniel R. Papaj 
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Teddy A. Wilkin, Ben C. Sheldon  Current Biology 
Biological components of sex differences in color preference
Merging of Long-Term Memories in an Insect
The nature of Drosophila melanogaster
Human sleep and cortical reactivity are influenced by lunar phase
Victoria F. Ratcliffe, David Reby  Current Biology 
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Janne-Tuomas Seppänen, Jukka T. Forsman  Current Biology 
G. Lorimer Moseley, Timothy J. Parsons, Charles Spence  Current Biology 
Addiction: Flies Hit the Skids
Mental Imagery Changes Multisensory Perception
Cognition and Evolution: Learning and the Evolution of Sex Traits
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Malcolm Burrows, Darron A. Cullen, Marina Dorosenko, Gregory P. Sutton 
Björn Brembs, Wolfgang Plendl  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System
High Resilience of Seed Dispersal Webs Highlighted by the Experimental Removal of the Dominant Disperser  Sérgio Timóteo, Jaime Albino Ramos, Ian Phillip.
DELLA-Mediated Cotyledon Expansion Breaks Coat-Imposed Seed Dormancy
Reinforcement Can Overcome Gene Flow during Speciation in Drosophila
Shifra Z. Goldenberg, Iain Douglas-Hamilton, George Wittemyer 
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Chimeric Synergy in Natural Social Groups of a Cooperative Microbe
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
Dynamics of a Memory Trace: Effects of Sleep on Consolidation
Bonobos Prefer Individuals that Hinder Others over Those that Help
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Neurogenetics: Short-Circuiting Sexually Dimorphic Behaviors
Source Memory in the Rat
Volume 26, Issue 4, Pages (February 2016)
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
A Taste Receptor Required for the Caffeine Response In Vivo
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Seasonal Encoding by the Circadian Pacemaker of the SCN
Peng Zhang, Min Bao, Miyoung Kwon, Sheng He, Stephen A. Engel 
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish
Malcolm Burrows, Darron A. Cullen, Marina Dorosenko, Gregory P. Sutton 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Motor Skills Are Strengthened through Reconsolidation
Clock and cycle Limit Starvation-Induced Sleep Loss in Drosophila
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Neil F. Milan, Balint Z. Kacsoh, Todd A. Schlenke  Current Biology 
Multisensory Integration and Attention in Developmental Dyslexia
FOXO transcription factors
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
Training Attentional Control in Infancy
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
Public Versus Personal Information for Mate Copying in an Invertebrate
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Active Inbreeding in a Cichlid Fish and Its Adaptive Significance
Social Facilitation of Long-Lasting Memory Retrieval in Drosophila
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Lysann Wagener, Maria Loconsole, Helen M. Ditz, Andreas Nieder 
Janne-Tuomas Seppänen, Jukka T. Forsman  Current Biology 
Impaired Associative Learning with Food Rewards in Obese Women
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Maria J.S. Guerreiro, Lisa Putzar, Brigitte Röder  Current Biology 
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Spread of Social Information and Dynamics of Social Transmission within Drosophila Groups  Marine Battesti, Celine Moreno, Dominique Joly, Frederic Mery  Current Biology  Volume 22, Issue 4, Pages 309-313 (February 2012) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050 Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Description of Aversive Oviposition Conditioning to Generate “Demonstrator” Females and Performance Indexes of Flies during the Two Test Phases After aversive oviposition conditioning, groups of ten flies were tested on fresh, flavored media without quinine over an 8 hr (0 hr–8 hr) and then a consecutive 16 hr (8 hr–24 hr) period. Yellow circles indicate banana-flavored media, pink circles indicate strawberry-flavored media, and empty circles indicate unflavored, pure agar media. Q indicates medium supplemented with quinine. Asterisks indicate a significant response to the conditioning: ∗∗∗p ≤ 10−3, ∗∗p ≤ 0.01, ∗p ≤ 0.05; n = 60 for all bars. Error bars represent ± standard errors of the mean (SEM). Current Biology 2012 22, 309-313DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Social Transmission of Oviposition Site Preference with Performance Indexes of Demonstrators and Observers Measured at the End of the Test Phase During the transmission phase, observer flies either had never experienced the oviposition media (A, B) or had already experienced the two flavored media (C). Transmission phase (B) was done in cages containing no flavored media. Circle colors are described in Figure 1. Asterisks indicate significant response to the initial conditioning (for demonstrators) or social transmission (for observers): ∗∗∗p ≤ 10−3, ∗∗p ≤ 0.01, ∗p ≤ 0.05. Observer groups without prior experience, n = 80; observer groups with prior experience, n = 78; observer groups in transmission phase without flavors, n = 56. Error bars represent ± standard errors of the mean (SEM). Current Biology 2012 22, 309-313DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Stability of Socially Acquired Information when Flies Have the Opportunity to Later Acquire Personal Information After a transmission phase, observer flies were introduced in cages containing either no oviposition media (A) or flavored media (B) for 4 hr. Circle colors are described in Figure 1. PIs were then calculated after an 8 hr test period in which all groups had access to flavored media. Asterisks indicate significant response to social interaction: ∗∗∗p ≤ 10−3, ∗∗p ≤ 0.01, ∗p ≤ 0.05. Current Biology 2012 22, 309-313DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Transmission of Social Information from First-Order Observers to Second-Order Observers with Performance Indexes of Demonstrators, First-Order Observers, and Second-Order Observers After conditioning, a group of demonstrators and a first group of naive observers were introduced into a new cage containing flavored media. First-order observers were then removed and introduced into a new cage with a group of second-order observers. Finally, all types of flies were placed in separate cages for a 12 hr test phase on fresh oviposition media. Circle colors are described in Figure 1. Asterisks indicate significant responses to the initial conditioning (for demonstrators) or social transmission (for observers): ∗∗∗p ≤ 10−3, ∗∗p ≤ 0.01, ∗p ≤ 0.05. Shown are (A) demonstrators groups, n = 74; (B) first-order observer groups, n = 74; (C) second-order observer groups, n = 74. Error bars represent ± standard errors of the mean (SEM). Current Biology 2012 22, 309-313DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.12.050) Copyright © 2012 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions