Volume 27, Issue 14, Pages e4 (July 2017)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Behavioural consequences of two chronic psychosocial stress paradigms: Anxiety without depression  David A. Slattery, Nicole Uschold, Mauro Magoni, Julia.
Advertisements

Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Copyright © 2009 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Sniffing Behavior Communicates Social Hierarchy
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Mark E.J. Sheffield, Michael D. Adoff, Daniel A. Dombeck  Neuron 
Mackenzie Weygandt Mathis, Alexander Mathis, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Spatial Memory Engram in the Mouse Retrosplenial Cortex
Sangyu Xu, Gishnu Das, Emily Hueske, Susumu Tonegawa  Current Biology 
Volume 138, Issue 4, Pages (April 2010)
Hiroaki Norimoto, Yuji Ikegaya  Current Biology 
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Nociceptive Sensitization Reduces Predation Risk
Social Jetlag and Obesity
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Volume 26, Issue 17, Pages (September 2016)
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Michael T. Rogan, Kam Sam Leon, David L. Perez, Eric R. Kandel  Neuron 
A Map for Social Navigation in the Human Brain
The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State
Bennett Drew Ferris, Jonathan Green, Gaby Maimon  Current Biology 
Nucleus Accumbens Neurons Are Innately Tuned for Rewarding and Aversive Taste Stimuli, Encode Their Predictors, and Are Linked to Motor Output  Mitchell.
Characterization of R and VR tumors grafted in mice.
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Volume 27, Issue 16, Pages e3 (August 2017)
Volume 73, Issue 4, Pages (February 2012)
Psychological Stress with Long-Standing Allergic Dermatitis Causes Psychodermatological Conditions in Mice  Hideki Kitagaki, Hidetaka Hiyama, Toshiki.
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e4 (October 2017)
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
Inversely Active Striatal Projection Neurons and Interneurons Selectively Delimit Useful Behavioral Sequences  Nuné Martiros, Alexandra A. Burgess, Ann.
Volume 26, Issue 7, Pages (April 2016)
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Kensaku Nomoto, Susana Q. Lima  Current Biology 
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Evolution of a Behavioral Shift Mediated by Superficial Neuromasts Helps Cavefish Find Food in Darkness  Masato Yoshizawa, Špela Gorički, Daphne Soares,
Inhibitory Interplay between Orexin Neurons and Eating
Volume 28, Issue 6, Pages e5 (March 2018)
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Behavioral and Neural Mechanisms of Overgeneralization in Anxiety
Guifen Chen, Daniel Manson, Francesca Cacucci, Thomas Joseph Wills 
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Signal Use by Octopuses in Agonistic Interactions
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
Volume 91, Issue 3, Pages (August 2016)
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Heidi C. Meyer, David J. Bucci  Current Biology 
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Volume 16, Issue 6, Pages (August 2016)
Volume 151, Issue 2, Pages (October 2012)
Left Habenular Activity Attenuates Fear Responses in Larval Zebrafish
Volume 23, Issue 12, Pages (June 2013)
Volume 37, Issue 6, Pages (December 2012)
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Volume 16, Issue 5, Pages (August 2016)
Sociobiology: Changing the Dominance Hierarchy
Volume 27, Issue 14, Pages e4 (July 2017)
Synchronized Activity between the Ventral Hippocampus and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Anxiety  Avishek Adhikari, Mihir A. Topiwala, Joshua A.
Volume 18, Issue 12, Pages (March 2017)
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (January 2017)
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
Volume 125, Issue 3, Pages (September 2003)
Volume 29, Issue 3, Pages e4 (February 2019)
Individual Recognition in Ant Queens
Volume 8, Issue 5, Pages (September 2014)
Volume 20, Issue 11, Pages (June 2010)
In vivo brain spectra acquired on a healthy volunteer by using a CT-PRESS sequence at 7T. In vivo brain spectra acquired on a healthy volunteer by using.
Presentation transcript:

Volume 27, Issue 14, Pages 2202-2210.e4 (July 2017) Hierarchical Status Predicts Behavioral Vulnerability and Nucleus Accumbens Metabolic Profile Following Chronic Social Defeat Stress  Thomas Larrieu, Antoine Cherix, Aranzazu Duque, João Rodrigues, Hongxia Lei, Rolf Gruetter, Carmen Sandi  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 14, Pages 2202-2210.e4 (July 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2017 27, 2202-2210.e4DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Hierarchical Rank Using a Social Confrontation Tube Test (A) Illustration of the general timeline of the study. (B) Example of one cage representing the tube test ranks and winning times as a function of tube test trials. (C) Summary for nine cages over the 6-day test trials. (D) Time spent in the tube (s) as a function of the rank pairing (F5,48 = 9.78, p < 0.001, one-way ANOVA; ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 9 per rank pairing). (E) Dominance score after agonistic behaviors in the homecage (t28 = 2.30, ∗p < 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 15 per group). (F) 2 × 2 contingency table for correlation between agonistic behaviors and tube test ranks (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.050). (G) Left: picture representing typical urine marks profile of dominant and subordinate mice revealed by a UV light source. Right: 2 × 2 contingency table for correlation between urine marking test and tube test ranks (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed, p = 0.026, n = 26 pairs). (H) Percentage of the time spent in the open arms (t34 = 2.33, ∗p < 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). (I) Percentage of the time spent in the closed arms (t34 = 2.19, ∗p < 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). (J) Latency for entering in the open arms (U = 105, p = 0.07, Mann-Whitney test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). (K) Percentage of the time spent in the center (t34 = 3.89, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). (L) Percentage of the time spent exploring the wall (thigmotaxis) (t34 = 1.86, p = 0.053, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). (M) Total distance traveled (t34 = 1.56, p > 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 18 per group). Right: representative heatmaps. (N–P) Pearson correlation coefficient (r) was calculated to establish relationships between individual social rank and basal anxiety score (N), between individual social rank and dominance score (O), and between basal anxiety and dominance behaviors (P). Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. See also Figures S1 and S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 2202-2210.e4DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Dominant Mice Exhibit Susceptible Phenotype after 10 Days of Social Defeat (A) Experimental design and representative tracking information of the time spent exploring a social target in control versus defeated dominant and subordinate mice. (B) Social avoidance scores for control and defeated dominant versus subordinate mice (F1,30 = 6.64, p < 0.05, two-way ANOVA; ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 8–9 per group). (C–E) Social status did not alter (C) the total duration of submissive behavior that defeated mice produced (t18 = 0.70, p > 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 10 per group), (D) the total duration of aggression that defeated mice received (t18 = 1.22, p > 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 10 per group), and (E) the latency to display defeat posture over the days (interaction: F2,20 = 0.26, p > 0.05; day effect: F2,20 = 3.85, p < 0.05; rank effect: F2,20 = 0.08, p > 0.05, two-way ANOVA). (F) Scores are calculated for each behavioral outcome. Social interaction ratios and time spent in interaction zone are multiplied by −1 so that higher scores reflect more social avoidance. The average of all four scores is used as the social avoidance score (t32 = 2.56, p = 0.015, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 16–18 per group). (G) Social avoidance score after segregation into resilient and susceptible defeated mice (stress effect: F2,33 = 31.47, p < 0.0001, one-way ANOVA; ∗∗∗p < 0.001 versus control group, Bonferroni’s test). (H) 2 × 2 contingency table analysis shows the proportion of dominant and subordinate mice that become resilient to CSDS (Fisher’s exact test, two-tailed ∗p = 0.0152). Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. See also Figure S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 2202-2210.e4DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Dominant and Subordinate Mice Exhibit Similar State Anxiety after CSDS (A) Time spent in the lit compartment of a light-dark box (interaction: F1,30 = 0.44, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,30 = 4.70, ∗p < 0.05; rank effect: F1,30 = 0.01, p > 0.05, two-way ANOVA, n = 8–9 per group). (B) Latency to enter the lit compartment of a light-dark box (interaction: F1,30 = 0.14, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,30 = 4.80, ∗p < 0.05; rank effect: F1,30 = 0.0018, p > 0.05, two-way ANOVA, n = 8–9 per group). (C) Representative heatmaps of time spent in the lit compartment of a light-dark box in control and defeated dominant versus subordinate mice. (D) Defeated dominant mice display an increase in the body weight during CSDS (interaction: F9,135 = 5.46, p < 0.0001, two-way ANOVA; ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, Bonferroni’s test, n = 8–9 per group). Dominant body weight on day 10 (t15 = 4.33, ∗∗∗p < 0.001, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 8–9 per group). (E) Body weight from defeated subordinate mice does not differ from control subordinate mice during CSDS (stress effect: F1,144 = 0.06, p > 0.05, two-way ANOVA, n = 8–10 per group). Subordinate body weight on day 10 (t16 = 0.02, p > 0.05, unpaired t test, two-tailed n = 8–10 per group). (F) Dominant and subordinate mice show similar free corticosterone response after CSDS (interaction: F1,30 = 2.29, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,30 = 8.13, ∗∗p < 0.01; rank effect: F1,30 = 1.69, p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA, n = 8–10 per group). (G) Free plasma testosterone after CSDS (interaction: F1,30 = 1.05, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,30 = 5.06, ∗p < 0.05; rank effect: F1,30 = 5.37, ∗p < 0.05; two-way ANOVA, n = 8–10 per group). (H) Testosterone/Corticosterone ratio after CSDS (interaction: F1,30 = 0.61, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,30 = 0.01, p > 0.05; rank effect: F1,30 = 3.56, p = 0.069; two-way ANOVA, n = 8–10 per group). Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. See also Figure S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 2202-2210.e4DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Factor Analysis Identified Two Main Factors that Account for the Variance in the Metabolic Data (A) Typical spectra and neuroanatomical images of mPFC and NAc with respective voxel localization acquired from a mouse. Spectra are shown with 2-Hz line broadening and two spectra are from the same mouse. Signals arising from the proton resonances used in this analysis are labeled as follows: N-acetylaspartate (NAA), myo-inositol (Ins), phosphocreatine (PCr), creatine (Cr), glutamate (Glu), glutamine (Gln), Glu + Gln (Glx), taurine (Tau), glycerophosphorylcholine (GPC), phosphorylcholine (PCho), total choline-containing compounds (tCho), γ-aminobutyrate (GABA), aspartate (Asp), glucose (Glc), ascorbate (Asc), N-acetylaspartylglutamate (NAAG), alanine (Ala), lactate (Lac), glycine (Gly), phosphatidylethanolamine (PE), and glutathione (GSH). (B) In the NAc, factor 1 represents a linear combination that summarizes metabolic changes found in metabolites with strong (above 0.6: PCr, Asp, Gln, Glu, Tau, Ins, Cr, and NAA) and moderate (0.4–0.6: GABA, PCho, PE, and GSH) contribution (interaction: F1,23 = 10.38, p < 0.01, two-way ANOVA; ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, Bonferroni’s test, n = 6–8 per group). (C) In the NAc, factor 2 represents a linear combination that summarizes metabolic changes found in metabolites with strong (above 0.6: NAAG and Asc) and moderate (0.4–0.6: PCr, Cr, and Glc) contribution (interaction: F1,23 = 0.73, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,23 = 0.07, p > 0.05; rank effect: F1,23 = 0.02, p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA, n = 6–8 per group). (D) In the mPFC, factor 1 represents a linear combination that summarizes metabolic changes found in metabolites with strong (above 0.6: PCr, GABA, GPC, GSH, Glu, Tau, Ins, Cr, and NAA) and moderate (0.4–0.6: Ala, Glc, Asp, Gln, NAAG, PE, and Asc) contribution (interaction: F1,23 = 1.41, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,23 = 0.04, p > 0.05; rank effect: F1,23 = 0.41, p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA, n = 6–8 per group). (E) In the mPFC, factor 2 represents a linear combination that summarizes metabolic changes found in metabolites with strong (above 0.6: Lac and Glc) and moderate (0.4–0.6: GPC) contribution (interaction: F1,23 = 0.05, p > 0.05; stress effect: F1,23 = 2.09, p > 0.05; rank effect: F1,23 = 1.10, p > 0.05; two-way ANOVA, n = 6–8 per group). Data are displayed as mean ± SEM. See also Figure S3 and Tables S1 and S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 2202-2210.e4DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.027) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions