I Know What You Are Doing

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Advertisements

Timing and Specificity of Feed-Forward Inhibition within the LGN
Volume 64, Issue 6, Pages (December 2009)
Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Interacting Roles of Attention and Visual Salience in V4
Ganesh Vigneswaran, Roland Philipp, Roger N. Lemon, Alexander Kraskov 
Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex
Uwe J Ilg, Stefan Schumann, Peter Thier  Neuron 
The Cortical Motor System
René Quilodran, Marie Rothé, Emmanuel Procyk  Neuron 
Kenway Louie, Matthew A. Wilson  Neuron 
Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys
Volume 52, Issue 3, Pages (November 2006)
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Value Representations in the Primate Striatum during Matching Behavior
Reaches to Sounds Encoded in an Eye-Centered Reference Frame
Goal-Related Activity in V4 during Free Viewing Visual Search
Attention-Induced Variance and Noise Correlation Reduction in Macaque V1 Is Mediated by NMDA Receptors  Jose L. Herrero, Marc A. Gieselmann, Mehdi Sanayei,
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 21, Issue 2, Pages (January 2011)
Inducing Gamma Oscillations and Precise Spike Synchrony by Operant Conditioning via Brain-Machine Interface  Ben Engelhard, Nofar Ozeri, Zvi Israel, Hagai.
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Activity in Posterior Parietal Cortex Is Correlated with the Relative Subjective Desirability of Action  Michael C. Dorris, Paul W. Glimcher  Neuron 
Lior Cohen, Gideon Rothschild, Adi Mizrahi  Neuron 
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Relationship of Correlated Spontaneous Activity to Functional Ocular Dominance Columns in the Developing Visual Cortex  Chiayu Chiu, Michael Weliky  Neuron 
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Eye Movements Modulate Visual Receptive Fields of V4 Neurons
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Volume 28, Issue 15, Pages e5 (August 2018)
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Peter Janssen, Rufin Vogels, Yan Liu, Guy A Orban  Neuron 
Slow-γ Rhythms Coordinate Cingulate Cortical Responses to Hippocampal Sharp-Wave Ripples during Wakefulness  Miguel Remondes, Matthew A. Wilson  Cell.
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Volume 42, Issue 2, Pages (April 2004)
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Masayuki Matsumoto, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Effects of Long-Term Visual Experience on Responses of Distinct Classes of Single Units in Inferior Temporal Cortex  Luke Woloszyn, David L. Sheinberg 
Volume 54, Issue 2, Pages (April 2007)
Genela Morris, David Arkadir, Alon Nevet, Eilon Vaadia, Hagai Bergman 
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Volume 24, Issue 14, Pages (July 2014)
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Wael F Asaad, Gregor Rainer, Earl K Miller  Neuron 
Sara E. Morrison, Alexandre Saez, Brian Lau, C. Daniel Salzman  Neuron 
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 37, Issue 3, Pages (February 2003)
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Population Responses to Contour Integration: Early Encoding of Discrete Elements and Late Perceptual Grouping  Ariel Gilad, Elhanan Meirovithz, Hamutal.
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Steven C. Leiser, Karen A. Moxon  Neuron 
A Temporal Channel for Information in Sparse Sensory Coding
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Dopamine Neurons Can Represent Context-Dependent Prediction Error
Presentation transcript:

I Know What You Are Doing M.A. Umiltà, E. Kohler, V. Gallese, L. Fogassi, L. Fadiga, C. Keysers, G. Rizzolatti  Neuron  Volume 31, Issue 1, Pages 155-165 (July 2001) DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3

Figure 1 Example of a Neuron Responding to Action Observation in Full Vision and in Hidden Condition but Not in Mimed Conditions The lower part of each panel illustrates schematically the experimenter's action as observed from the monkey's vantage point: the experimenter's hand started from a fixed position, moved toward an object, and grasped it (A and B) or mimed grasping (C and D). The black frame depicts the metallic frame interposed between the experimenter and the monkey in all conditions. In (B) and (D), the gray square inside the black frame represents the opaque sliding screen that prevented the monkey from seeing the action that the experimenter performed behind it. The asterisk indicates the location of a stationary marker attached to the frame. In hidden conditions, the experimenter's hand started to disappear from the monkey's vision when crossing this marker. The behavioral paradigm consisted of two basic conditions: full vision condition (A) and hidden condition (B). Two further conditions were performed: miming in full vision (C) and hidden miming (D). In these last two conditions, the monkey observed the same movements as in (A) and (B) but without a target object. In each panel above the illustration of the corresponding experimenter's hand movements, raster displays and histograms of ten consecutive trials recorded during these hand movements are shown. Above each raster, a colored line represents the kinematics of the experimenter's hand movements, expressed as the distance between the hand of the experimenter and the stationary marker over time. The colored bars in the rasters represent landmarks of the hand movement of the experimenter in that particular trial: the onset of the hand movement (green bars), the crossing of the stationary marker (red bars), the touching of the object target of the action, or, in trials without objects, the touching of the location at which the objects were otherwise placed (pale blue bars). Rasters and histograms are aligned with the moment at which the experimenter's hand was closest to the fixed marker (minimum in the kinematics, red bars in the rasters, and dashed vertical line in the histograms). To illustrate how similar the kinematics were between conditions, in the bottom line and rightmost column, the hand kinematics shown in the rasters are overlaid and magnified in the vertical dimension while preserving their temporal scale and alignment. Each trace has the same color in the raster panel and in the overlay panel, allowing identification of the corresponding condition. The illustrated neuron responded to the observation of grasping and holding in full vision (A) and in the hidden condition (B) in which the interaction between the experimenter's hand and the object occurred behind the opaque screen. The neuron response was virtually absent in the two conditions in which the observed action was mimed (C and D). See text for statistical results. Histograms bin width = 20 ms. The scale of the abscissa is identical for all elements of the illustration. The ordinate is in spikes/s for histograms and centimeters for the overlaid kinematics Neuron 2001 31, 155-165DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3)

Figure 2 Further Example of a Mirror Neuron that Responded to Action Observation in Full Vision and in Hidden Condition but Not in Mimed Conditions This neuron responded to the observation of grasping and holding in full vision (A) and in hidden condition (B). (C) and (D) show the discharge of the same neuron in miming in full vision and hidden miming conditions, respectively. All other conventions are as in Figure 1. Note that the hand kinematics saturated in the initial 250 ms but overlapped extremely well thereafter Neuron 2001 31, 155-165DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3)

Figure 3 Population Response in Full Vision and Hidden Condition For the early movement, late movement, and holding epochs, the net normalized activity is shown, averaged (±SEM) over all 37 tested neurons of the population. This is done separately for the full vision and hidden condition as solid and interrupted lines, respectively. Due to the normalization (see text for details), the net activity is always 0 during the background epoch. An asterisk signifies that the activity of the population during this epoch is significantly higher than that during the background epoch of the same condition. A cross next to a bracket signifies that the activity of the population during that epoch is significantly different between the two conditions. (All p < 0.01 according to a Newman-Keuls post-hoc analysis, see text for details.) Neuron 2001 31, 155-165DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3)

Figure 4 Examples of Mirror Neurons, Showing Different Types of Responses The responses of three mirror neurons are shown to illustrate three types of responses. All rasters and histograms are aligned on the moment in which the hand of the experimenter (all except [E1]) or the monkey (E1) touched the object or, in conditions without objects, the empty plate where the object is otherwise placed. All histograms share the same scale. Neuron 1 showed a strong response to grasping and holding observation in full vision (A1). The response, although present, was markedly reduced in hidden condition (B1). In (C1) and (D1), the experimenter placed an object on a plane and held it there for 1 s, in full vision (C1) and in hidden condition (D1). Note the specificity of the neuron response manifested by the very low intensity of the discharge during placing and holding observation in both conditions. (E1) shows the response of the neuron during the monkey's active grasping of a small piece of food, with the hand contralateral to the recorded hemisphere. Neuron 2 responded to grasping and holding observation in full vision (A2) and in hidden condition (B2). In full vision (A2), the neuron started to discharge during the late grasping phase and increased its firing during holding. In hidden condition (B2), the neuron started to respond as soon as the experimenter's hand disappeared from the monkey's sight, reached the discharge peak intensity at the end of grasping, and decreased during holding. (C2) and (D2) show the response of the same neuron in the two mimed conditions. Note the almost complete absence of a response in these conditions. Neuron 3 showed a strong response during the observation of the experimenter's grasping and holding actions (A3). The same actions, when hidden, did not evoke any response (B3) Neuron 2001 31, 155-165DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3)

Figure 5 Eye Position during the Observation of Hand Actions in All Four Conditions of the Pilot Experiment The mean distance in degrees (±SEM) between the point of gaze and the object/plane location is shown at six times (−500 ms to 0 ms) relative to the moment at which the experimenter's hand touches the object/plane. While at the moment of touching, no significant difference exists between conditions; in the 500 ms to 300 ms preceding touching, only in the full vision condition, the monkey was gazing at the object location. At each of the six points in time, a four-condition ANOVA with ten trials per condition was performed. The results indicate a significant main effect of condition at −500 ms F3,36 = 4, p < 0.02, −400 ms F3,36 = 16, p < 0.01, and −300 ms F3,36 = 6, p < 0.01; a trend at −200 ms F3,36 = 2, p = 0.13; and no main effect at −100 ms F3,36 = 0.45, p > 0.7 or at the moment of touching F3,36 = 0.23, p > 0.8. Asterisks indicate significant differences (p < 0.05) Neuron 2001 31, 155-165DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00337-3)