Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
New Eye-Tracking Techniques May Revolutionize Mental Health Screening
Advertisements

Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Mackenzie Weygandt Mathis, Alexander Mathis, Naoshige Uchida  Neuron 
Motor Task Variation Induces Structural Learning
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Using a Compound Gain Field to Compute a Reach Plan
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Coding of the Reach Vector in Parietal Area 5d
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Generating Coherent Patterns of Activity from Chaotic Neural Networks
Braden A. Purcell, Roozbeh Kiani  Neuron 
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Scale-Invariant Movement Encoding in the Human Motor System
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Syed A. Chowdhury, Gregory C. DeAngelis  Neuron 
Volume 75, Issue 1, Pages (July 2012)
Jack Grinband, Joy Hirsch, Vincent P. Ferrera  Neuron 
Dynamic Coding for Cognitive Control in Prefrontal Cortex
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Jonathan J. Nassi, David C. Lyon, Edward M. Callaway  Neuron 
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Optogenetic Inactivation Modifies Monkey Visuomotor Behavior
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Neural Mechanisms of Visual Motion Perception in Primates
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Intact Memory for Irrelevant Information Impairs Perception in Amnesia
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Optic Ataxia: From Balint’s Syndrome to the Parietal Reach Region
Redmond G. O’Connell, Michael N. Shadlen, KongFatt Wong-Lin, Simon P
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Volume 77, Issue 6, Pages (March 2013)
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco, David J. Heeger, Justin L. Gardner 
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Volume 23, Issue 3, Pages (April 2018)
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 88, Issue 4, Pages (November 2015)
Neural and Computational Mechanisms of Action Processing: Interaction between Visual and Motor Representations  Martin A. Giese, Giacomo Rizzolatti  Neuron 
Motor Planning, Not Execution, Separates Motor Memories
What to Do, or How to Do It? Neuron
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Fruit-Catching Fish Tune Their Fast Starts to Compensate for Drift
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Attention, Learning, and the Value of Information
New Eye-Tracking Techniques May Revolutionize Mental Health Screening
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Richard A. Andersen, He Cui  Neuron 
Human Posterior Parietal Cortex Flexibly Determines Reference Frames for Reaching Based on Sensory Context  Pierre-Michel Bernier, Scott T. Grafton  Neuron 
Valerio Mante, Vincent Bonin, Matteo Carandini  Neuron 
James N. Ingram, Ian S. Howard, J. Randall Flanagan, Daniel M. Wolpert 
Visual Crowding at a Distance during Predictive Remapping
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Computational Mechanisms of Sensorimotor Control David W. Franklin, Daniel M. Wolpert  Neuron  Volume 72, Issue 3, Pages 425-442 (November 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 OFC Predicts Adaptive Feedback Responses (A) Paths of reaching movement in the horizontal plane viewed from above. Although moving toward a target (central black circle), the target location could jump laterally (top or bottom targets) at three different times after movement start. These produce different feedback responses depending on the timing of the target shift. (B) Speed profiles for the movements shown in (A). (C) Lateral deviation (toward the displaced target and perpendicular to main movement direction). Subjects make different corrective movements depending on the time of the target shift. (D) An OFC model predicts similar trajectories to human subjects. (E) The complex time varying feedback gains of the optimal feedback controller that give rise to the predicted movements. kp, accuracy of final position; kv, limiting movement velocity; ka, limiting activation; max, maximum. (F and G) Speed and lateral deviation predicted by the optimal control model. Similar to human subjects, the model predicts that when time allows, minimal corrective movements are made to reach the shifted target (low gains in E). On the other hand, when the target jump occurs late in the movement, both the speed and lateral movement are changed, and some undershoot of the movement trajectory is produced (adapted with permission from Liu and Todorov [2007]). Neuron 2011 72, 425-442DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Forward Models in the Sensorimotor System: Evidence from the Saccadic System (A) The neural pathway investigated between the visual brain areas and the motor system responsible for saccades. A major visual to motor pathway (blue arrows) takes information from the ventral and dorsal streams, via lateral intraparietal area (LIP) to the FEF. The FEF conveys information to the SC, which triggers saccades via nuclei within the brain stem. The saccadic system also has a motor to visual pathway (red arrows), which was proposed to carry corollary discharge (CD) of the motor actions from the SC to the FEF via the MD of the thalamus. In a series of experiments, Sommer and Wurtz (2002, 2004b, 2006) inactivated the MD relay neurons by injecting muscimol to test if this pathway provided motor information about impending eye saccades to higher visual areas (FEF) similar to forward models. (B) The double-step saccade task. Although the eye is fixating the starting location (gray circle), two targets (orange circles) are flashed briefly that need to be fixated in turn. No visual feedback is present during either of the two saccades. The location of the second target is only available as a vector from the original staring location. Once the eye has made a saccade to the first target, information from the motor system about the movement that occurred is used to update the location of the second target relative to current eye fixation position. If this corollary discharge information is lost, then the position of the second saccade would not be updated, and the second saccade would miss the target (dotted arrow). (C) When the SC-MD-FEF pathway is intact, saccades to the first target exhibit a spread of endpoints (gray ellipse) due to noise; however, the second saccades are angled appropriately to compensate for the variation in starting postures. (D) When the MD replay neurons have been inactivated, the second saccade is not directed at the appropriate angle to the second target. Moreover, the trial-by-trial variations in the endpoint of the first saccade were not compensated for accurately in the second saccade (Sommer and Wurtz, 2002, 2004b). (E) Shifts of the FEF receptive field occur prior to saccade. When fixating (blue cross), an FEF neuron has a receptive field within a particular location (green circle); however, just before a saccade, the receptive field of the neuron shifts to its future location (purple circle). After the saccade, this new location is the receptive field. (F) With an intact SC-MD-FEF pathway (red), probes in the receptive field location elicit neural firing prior to the saccade to the new location (upper plots). Moreover, probes in the future receptive field also elicit neural firing directly prior to the saccade (lower plots), indicating that the receptive field shifts prior to the saccade. When the MD is inactivated (blue), the neural firing in the receptive field is unaffected, but significant deficits (light-blue region) were found to probes in the future receptive field (F adapted by permission from Macmillan Publishers Ltd: Nature, Sommer and Wurtz [2006]). Neuron 2011 72, 425-442DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Error-Based Learning Algorithm Predicts Changes in Muscle Activation and Coactivation (A) We consider a single-joint motion for simplicity. In this example, the elbow joint is controlled by two antagonist muscles, the biceps (red) and triceps (blue). During a movement the current joint angle is compared to the desired joint angle to give rise to a sequence of errors. (B) Each error measure is used by the V-shaped update rule to determine the change in muscle activation for the next repetition of the movement. This change in muscle activation is shifted forward in time on the subsequent trial to compensate for the delays. The V-shaped learning rule for each muscle (biceps, red; triceps, blue) has a different slope depending on whether the error indicates that the muscle is too long or too short at each point in time. (C) The different slopes for stretch or shortening of each muscle lead to an appropriate change in the reciprocal muscle activation that drives compensatory changes in the joint torques and endpoint forces. (D) Large errors lead to an increase in coactivation, whereas little or no error leads to a reduction in the coactivation. (E) Changes in muscle activation patterns during repeated trials. In the steady-state condition a movement may be produced by coordinated feedforward activation of an agonist-antagonist pair (trial 0). The introduction of a disturbance that extends the elbow joint (trial 1) produces a large feedback response in the biceps muscle (dark red). On the subsequent trial (trial 2), the learning algorithm changes both the biceps and triceps activation pattern (shifted forward in time from the feedback response). Due to this action, a smaller feedback response is produced on this trial. In the next trial (trial 3), the feedforward activation is again increased based on the error on the previous trial such that the disturbance is compensated for perfectly. This leads to a reduction in the coactivation on the next trial (trial 4). (B)–(D) were redrawn with permission from Franklin et al. (2008). Neuron 2011 72, 425-442DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.006) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions