Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages R1200-R1206 (November 2017)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Ancient Endo-siRNA Pathways Reveal New Tricks Julie M. Claycomb Current Biology Volume 24, Issue 15, Pages R703-R715 (August 2014) DOI: /j.cub
Advertisements

Deepening the darkness? Alfred Russel Wallace in the Malay Archipelago
Emotional Memory: Selective Enhancement by Sleep
Volume 27, Issue 11, Pages R447-R448 (June 2017)
Evolution: When Dinosaurs Bested Their Early Rivals
Volume 21, Issue 21, Pages R894-R896 (November 2011)
Prey–Predator Communication: For Your Sensors Only
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Spatial Memory: Mice Quickly Learn a Safe Haven
Synaptic Physiology: Illuminating the Road Ahead
Human Memory: Brain-State-Dependent Effects of Stimulation
Social Behaviour: The Personalities of Groups
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Predatory grasshopper mice
Comparative Cognition: Action Imitation Using Episodic Memory
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Microbiology: Mixing Wine, Chocolate, and Coffee
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
Life History: The Energy-Efficient Orangutan
Evolution: The Mystery of Imperfect Mimicry
Neurobiology: Jumping Spiders Getting On Board
Infant cognition Current Biology
Host–Microbe Interactions: Winning the Colonization Lottery
Evolution: One Penis After All
Plant Grafting: Making the Right Connections
Sleep: How Many Switches Does It Take To Turn Off the Lights?
Volume 24, Issue 5, Pages R204-R206 (March 2014)
Homing Behavior: Decisions, Dominance and Democracy
Evolution: One Penis After All
Houses made by protists
Professor Jekyll and Comrade Hyde
Volume 25, Issue 23, Pages R1114-R1116 (December 2015)
American birds: Audubon was not the first
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Volume 23, Issue 9, Pages R364-R365 (May 2013)
An Embodied View of Octopus Neurobiology
Evolution: Mirror, Mirror in the Pond
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Fifty years of illumination about the natural levels of adaptation
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Volume 24, Issue 7, Pages R262-R263 (March 2014)
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages R88-R89 (February 2017)
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Elementary motion detectors
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
It’s all about the constraints
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Badger social networks correlate with tuberculosis infection
Volume 15, Issue 13, Pages R483-R484 (July 2005)
Magic and cognitive neuroscience
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Stephen G. Lomber, Blake E. Butler  Current Biology 
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Septins: Cellular and Functional Barriers of Neuronal Activity
Tool Use: Crows Craft the Right Tool for the Job
FOXO transcription factors
Neuronal Plasticity: How Do Neurons Know What To Do?
Improving management of neonatal infections
Volume 19, Issue 20, Pages R922-R923 (November 2009)
Horizontal Gene Transfer: Accidental Inheritance Drives Adaptation
Grasping Weber's law Current Biology
American birds: Audubon was not the first
Piezo channels Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages R1200-R1206 (November 2017) Habits  T.W. Robbins, Rui M. Costa  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages R1200-R1206 (November 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.060 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Habits and goal-directed actions. (A) Example of a sequence of stimulus, response or action, and outcome. Someone sees a light switch, flips the light switch, and the light comes on. (B) Goal-directed actions are performed in order to obtain the future outcome. If the outcome (light coming on) becomes less valuable — for example, if the environment is already well illuminated (outcome devaluation) — the action will not be performed. Similarly, if the contingency between the action (flipping the switch) and the outcome (light on) is reduced — for example, an automatic switch is installed and the light comes on in the absence of the action (contingency degradation) — the action will not be performed. (C) Habits are elicited by antecedent stimuli and not performed to obtain future outcomes. Therefore, the response (flipping the switch) will be performed upon sight of the switch (stimulus), even if the outcome has been devalued or the relation between action and outcome degraded. Current Biology 2017 27, R1200-R1206DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.060) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 The Creature of Habit Questionnaire reveals habits in everyday life. The nodes represent the individual items of the questionnaire. The thickness of lines connecting nodes is proportional to the size of corresponding correlation. The colors represent groupings of items of a subscale, as identified by Mokken Scale Analysis. Examples of routines and automaticities are given (adapted from Ersche et al. 2017 and reproduced by permission of author and publisher). Current Biology 2017 27, R1200-R1206DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.060) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Sequences can become habits or routines. (A) Example of a situation where a sequence of actions leads to the ultimate outcome. Upon sight of a dirty hand (stimulus), the faucet is turned on, the hands are washed and then dried, resulting in clean hands. (B) The sequence of actions can become automatized, with one action eliciting the next, and the entire sequence can become a response to the stimulus (represented hierarchically or not). This automatization makes it more difficult to stop the sequence in the middle, or to adapt to circumstances in which one of the elements in the sequence is not necessary — for example, if the faucet has a motion sensor. (C) The routine of washing hands can become reinforcing and a goal in itself. In this case, the completion of the routine — that is, achieving the last step in the sequence — may reinforce or drive the entire response. Current Biology 2017 27, R1200-R1206DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.09.060) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions