Susanne Åkesson, Jens Morin, Rachel Muheim, Ulf Ottosson 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Intro to Map and Compass
Advertisements

Geomagnetic field Inclination
Section 1: Finding Locations on EarthFinding Locations on Earth
Models of the Earth Chapter 3. Ch03\80017.html.
Models of the Earth Section 1 Preview Key Ideas Latitude Longitude Comparing Latitude and Longitude Great Circles Finding Direction Section 1: Finding.
Chapter 3 Objectives Distinguish between latitude and longitude.
A snapshot of the 3D magnetic field structure simulated with the Glatzmaier-Roberts geodynamo model. Magnetic field lines are blue where the field is directed.
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Sensory bases of navigation
Henrik Mouritsen, Gesa Feenders, Miriam Liedvogel, Wiebke Kropp 
Volume 19, Issue 24, Pages (December 2009)
Annual 10-Month Aerial Life Phase in the Common Swift Apus apus
Volume 14, Issue 6, Pages R221-R224 (March 2004)
Earth and its coordinates
Volume 23, Issue 4, Pages (February 2013)
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Hiroaki Norimoto, Yuji Ikegaya  Current Biology 
Coldness Triggers Northward Flight in Remigrant Monarch Butterflies
Volume 21, Issue 18, Pages (September 2011)
How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward
Volume 26, Issue 24, Pages R1266-R1267 (December 2016)
MRI Magnetic Field Stimulates Rotational Sensors of the Brain
Volume 17, Issue 24, Pages (December 2007)
Tornadic Storm Avoidance Behavior in Breeding Songbirds
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e2 (September 2017)
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
The Role of the Magnetite-Based Receptors in the Beak in Pigeon Homing
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages R1152-R1153 (November 2017)
Keratocyte Fragments and Cells Utilize Competing Pathways to Move in Opposite Directions in an Electric Field  Yaohui Sun, Hao Do, Jing Gao, Ren Zhao,
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Climate-Mediated Movement of an Avian Hybrid Zone
Walking Straight into Circles
Context-Dependent Decay of Motor Memories during Skill Acquisition
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation
Martin Bringmann, Dominique C. Bergmann  Current Biology 
Context-dependent lateralized feeding strategies in blue whales
Longitude Perception and Bicoordinate Magnetic Maps in Sea Turtles
Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)
Annual 10-Month Aerial Life Phase in the Common Swift Apus apus
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
A Magnetic Map Leads Juvenile European Eels to the Gulf Stream
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Box Jellyfish Use Terrestrial Visual Cues for Navigation
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
CA3 Retrieves Coherent Representations from Degraded Input: Direct Evidence for CA3 Pattern Completion and Dentate Gyrus Pattern Separation  Joshua P.
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Rémi Bos, Christian Gainer, Marla B. Feller  Current Biology 
Walter Jetz, Dustin R. Rubenstein  Current Biology 
Keram Pfeiffer, Uwe Homberg  Current Biology 
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Nikita Chernetsov, Dmitry Kishkinev, Henrik Mouritsen  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 7, Pages (April 2008)
Evidence that Magnetic Navigation and Geomagnetic Imprinting Shape Spatial Genetic Variation in Sea Turtles  J. Roger Brothers, Kenneth J. Lohmann  Current.
Volume 19, Issue 24, Pages (December 2009)
Daniel E. Winkowski, Eric I. Knudsen  Neuron 
Tracking Route Progression in the Posterior Parietal Cortex
Volume 23, Issue 24, Pages R1083-R1085 (December 2013)
Path Integration: Combining Optic Flow with Compass Orientation
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Humans Can Continuously Optimize Energetic Cost during Walking
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Volume 21, Issue 23, Pages (December 2011)
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
The Geography of Ecological Niche Evolution in Mammals
Eurasian reed warblers compensate for virtual magnetic displacement
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Dramatic Orientation Shift of White-Crowned Sparrows Displaced across Longitudes in the High Arctic  Susanne Åkesson, Jens Morin, Rachel Muheim, Ulf Ottosson  Current Biology  Volume 15, Issue 17, Pages 1591-1597 (September 2005) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.027 Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Sites Where Orientation Cage Experiments with White-Crowned Sparrows Were Performed in Autumn, 1999 The birds were captured in the breeding area (site 1), and their orientation was recorded in circular cages at sites 1–9. At one site (open circle), the birds were transported to the tundra, but no experiments were performed because of high wind speeds. In 1999, the geomagnetic North Pole was located at Ellef Ringnes Island (site 5, arrow in [A]). (A) The map is a polar stereographic projection and gives the magnetic declinations in accordance with the World Magnetic Model/Chart, Epoch 2000. Declination isolines in yellow denote positive (deviations to the east of geographic north) and red negative (deviations to the west) values, respectively. (B) The map gives isoclinics (geomagnetic inclination) as red (broken) lines and isodynamics (total field intensity, µT) as blue (filled) lines in accordance with the International Geomagnetic Reference Field (IGRF 2000) across the North American continent. The star indicates the site of capture. Current Biology 2005 15, 1591-1597DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.027) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Expected Courses as a Response to Longitudinal Displacement Expected courses as a response to displacements eastward across longitudes in high-arctic North America to sites east (sites 6–9) of the magnetic North Pole were calculated on the basis of the observed mean orientation for juvenile white-crowned sparrows recorded at the western sites (sites 2–4). Expected courses are presented at sites east (clear skies, overcast, null magnetic field) of the magnetic North Pole for different orientation mechanisms. (A) Constant geographic course at all sites. (B) Constant magnetic course at all sites. (C) Magnetic compass courses recalibrated on the basis of solar information, demonstrated under overcast skies, and resulting in a magnetic course shifted toward south (to the right) after recalibration. (D) Celestial compass courses recalibrated on the basis of magnetic information and shown in a null magnetic field where magnetic compass information is absent. (E) Expected courses from each experimental site (number refers to sites in Table 1) to center of expected wintering area as calculated for an initial great-circle route (i.e., shortest route between two points). Ranges of directions along rhumbline routes (i.e., constant compass course) for the same sites are 148°–178° from sites west of the magnetic North Pole (sites 2–4) and 190°–216° for sites east (sites 6–9). (F) Expected courses from each experimental site (indicated by numbers) to site of capture as initial great-circle routes (ranges of directions for rhumbline routes from the same sites are from west sites, 119°–230°, and east sites, 240°–280°). Current Biology 2005 15, 1591-1597DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.027) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Orientation of Displaced White-Crowned Sparrows in High-Arctic North America Orientations of individual birds were recorded in cages under clear skies (adults: [A–C], juveniles: [H–J]) and simulated overcast (adults: [D–G], juveniles: [K–N]) in autumn at nine tundra sites (eight presented in the graph), in the breeding area (site 1), and after longitudinal displacement in western (sites 2–4) and eastern (sites 6–9) high-arctic North America. Data from the magnetic North Pole (site 5) are not included but are presented in Table 1. The symbols (filled, unimodal; open, axial) inside the circles indicate the grand mean orientation of the experimental birds as calculated for a number of tests at several sites, and mean orientation for single tests at different sites are given outside the circles. The calculated migratory directions along an initial great-circle route (i.e., shortest route between two sites, GC; open arrowhead) and rhumbline route (i.e., constant compass course, RL; filled arrowhead) from the breeding site to the wintering area are indicated in (A) and (H), and the mean directions (GC, open arrowheads; RL, filled arrowheads) from eastern sites (sites 6–9) toward the site of capture are indicated in (C), (E), (G), (J), (L), and (N). The length of the arrow (r) is a measure of the scatter of the circular distribution, ranging from 0 to 1 and being inversely related to scatter [17]. Mean angle of orientation (given in degrees), vector length (r), and significance levels (indicated as ns, p > 0.05; *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01; and ***, p < 0.001) according to the Rayleigh test [17] are given for the distributions inside the circles. Distributions for experiments under simulated overcast skies at sites west and east of the magnetic North Pole are plotted both relative to geographic north (west, ad in [D] and juv in [K]; east, ad in [E] and juv in [L]), and magnetic north (west, ad in [F] and juv in [M]; east, ad in [G] and juv in [N]). Ninety-five percent confidence limits are given as hatched lines. Vectors for distributions not significantly different from random are indicated by a broken line. Mean direction toward the sun in the middle of the test period is given for experiments under clear-sky conditions. Current Biology 2005 15, 1591-1597DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2005.07.027) Copyright © 2005 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions