Volume 88, Issue 6, Pages (December 2015)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Neurobiology of Decision: Consensus and Controversy Joseph W. Kable, Paul W. Glimcher Neuron Volume 63, Issue 6, Pages (September 2009) DOI:
Advertisements

Preference Distributions of Primary Motor Cortex Neurons Reflect Control Solutions Optimized for Limb Biomechanics Timothy P. Lillicrap, Stephen H. Scott.
The Pathobiology of Vascular Dementia Costantino Iadecola Neuron Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages (November 2013) DOI: /j.neuron Copyright.
Decision Making as a Window on Cognition Michael N. Shadlen, Roozbeh Kiani Neuron Volume 80, Issue 3, Pages (October 2013) DOI: /j.neuron
Contour Saliency in Primary Visual Cortex Wu Li, Valentin Piëch, Charles D. Gilbert Neuron Volume 50, Issue 6, Pages (June 2006) DOI: /j.neuron
Neuronal Cell Types and Connectivity: Lessons from the Retina H. Sebastian Seung, Uygar Sümbül Neuron Volume 83, Issue 6, Pages (September 2014)
Single Units in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex with Anxiety-Related Firing Patterns Are Preferentially Influenced by Ventral Hippocampal Activity Avishek.
Pyramidal Neurons Grow Up and Change Their Mind Gord Fishell, Carina Hanashima Neuron Volume 57, Issue 3, Pages (February 2008) DOI: /j.neuron
Hippocampal Activity Patterns Carry Information about Objects in Temporal Context Liang-Tien Hsieh, Matthias J. Gruber, Lucas J. Jenkins, Charan Ranganath.
Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others Jason P. Mitchell, C. Neil Macrae, Mahzarin R. Banaji Neuron.
Prior cocaine exposure disrupts extinction of fear conditioning
Volume 65, Issue 6, Pages (March 2010)
Building Better Models of Visual Cortical Receptive Fields
Risk-Responsive Orbitofrontal Neurons Track Acquired Salience
How Did the Chicken Cross the Road
Sparse Encoding of Natural Scents
Volume 46, Issue 3, Pages (May 2005)
Limber Neurons for a Nimble Mind
Shih-Chieh Lin, Miguel A.L. Nicolelis  Neuron 
Rachel Ludmer, Yadin Dudai, Nava Rubin  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Volume 59, Issue 4, Pages (August 2008)
Daphna Shohamy, Anthony D. Wagner  Neuron 
Volume 90, Issue 6, Pages (June 2016)
Volume 96, Issue 5, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Volume 84, Issue 1, Pages (October 2014)
Listening for the Right Sounds
Volume 63, Issue 1, Pages (July 2009)
Sparse Encoding of Natural Scents
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
The Orbitofrontal Oracle: Cortical Mechanisms for the Prediction and Evaluation of Specific Behavioral Outcomes  Peter H. Rudebeck, Elisabeth A. Murray 
Developmental Inhibition of Gsk3 Rescues Behavioral and Neurophysiological Deficits in a Mouse Model of Schizophrenia Predisposition  Makoto Tamura, Jun.
Activity in Both Hippocampus and Perirhinal Cortex Predicts the Memory Strength of Subsequently Remembered Information  Yael Shrager, C. Brock Kirwan,
R. Ellen Ambrose, Brad E. Pfeiffer, David J. Foster  Neuron 
Volume 77, Issue 5, Pages (March 2013)
Volume 54, Issue 1, Pages (April 2007)
Adaptive Training Diminishes Distractibility in Aging across Species
Volume 65, Issue 6, Pages (March 2010)
Learning to Link Visual Contours
NMDA Receptors in Dopaminergic Neurons Are Crucial for Habit Learning
A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its Implications
Volume 59, Issue 2, Pages (July 2008)
Rachel Yehuda, Joseph LeDoux  Neuron 
Peter Kok, Janneke F.M. Jehee, Floris P. de Lange  Neuron 
Neuronal Activity in Primate Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Signals Task Conflict and Predicts Adjustments in Pupil-Linked Arousal  R. Becket Ebitz,
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Heidi C. Meyer, David J. Bucci  Current Biology 
Hippocampal and Ventral Medial Prefrontal Activation during Retrieval-Mediated Learning Supports Novel Inference  Dagmar Zeithamova, April L. Dominick,
Uma R. Karmarkar, Dean V. Buonomano  Neuron 
Sharon C. Furtak, Omar J. Ahmed, Rebecca D. Burwell  Neuron 
Volume 90, Issue 6, Pages (June 2016)
Dopamine: Burning the Candle at Both Ends
Value-Based Modulations in Human Visual Cortex
Extinction Generates Outcome-Specific Conditioned Inhibition
Volume 91, Issue 6, Pages (September 2016)
Volume 90, Issue 6, Pages (June 2016)
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
Sarah R. Heilbronner, Michael L. Platt  Neuron 
Michael J. Frank, Brion S. Woroch, Tim Curran  Neuron 
Movement Coding at the Mesoscale in Posterior Parietal Cortex
Vinay Parikh, Rouba Kozak, Vicente Martinez, Martin Sarter  Neuron 
Temporal Specificity of Reward Prediction Errors Signaled by Putative Dopamine Neurons in Rat VTA Depends on Ventral Striatum  Yuji K. Takahashi, Angela J.
Paying Attention to the Details of Attention
Volume 70, Issue 1, Pages (April 2011)
Similarity Breeds Proximity: Pattern Similarity within and across Contexts Is Related to Later Mnemonic Judgments of Temporal Proximity  Youssef Ezzyat,
Orbitofrontal Cortex as a Cognitive Map of Task Space
Distributed Coding of Actual and Hypothetical Outcomes in the Orbital and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex  Hiroshi Abe, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron  Volume 70,
David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Updates Its Status
Michael A. Silver, Amitai Shenhav, Mark D'Esposito  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Volume 88, Issue 6, Pages 1268-1280 (December 2015) Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex Mediates Outcome Retrieval in Partially Observable Task Situations  Laura A. Bradfield, Amir Dezfouli, Mieke van Holstein, Billy Chieng, Bernard W. Balleine  Neuron  Volume 88, Issue 6, Pages 1268-1280 (December 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 mOFC Lesions Impair Specific-PIT (A) In the Pavlovian phase, rats were trained to associate two stimuli, S1 and S2, with pellet and sucrose outcomes (O1 and O2, counterbalanced). In the instrumental phase, the same rats were trained to associate two lever press responses (A1 and A2) with the same pellet and sucrose outcomes (A1-O1 and A2-O2). On test, each stimulus was presented separately and rats were given a choice between levers (A1 versus A2). The lever presses were recorded, but no outcomes were delivered. (B) Representation of lesion placements showing each overlapping cytotoxic lesion. (C) Simulated predictions for the responding of sham/control and lesion animals in specific-PIT. The error bars are present, but negligible on this, and all the simulation figures (OA = other action; i.e., any action other than the target actions). (D) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) during specific-PIT test for sham and lesioned animals. Neuron 2015 88, 1268-1280DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 mOFC Lesions Impair Instrumental Outcome Devaluation Performance (A) The instrumental phase is identical to Figure 1A (A1-O1 and A2-O2). For the devaluation, the rats were sated for 1 hr on one outcome, i.e., O1 (1 hr) prior to a choice test, A1 versus A2. (B) Simulated predictions for the responding of sham/control animals in outcome devaluation (OA = 0.33 for Sham and OA = 0.36 for Lesioned animals) (data not shown). (C) Mean percentage of baseline responding (±1 SEM) during outcome devaluation (in extinction) for sham and lesioned animals. (D) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) during outcome devaluation for sham and lesioned animals on a rewarded test. Neuron 2015 88, 1268-1280DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 mOFC Lesions Spare Outcome-Selective Performance (A) For outcome-selective reinstatement: the instrumental phase is identical to Figure 1A. The rats were then given 15 min extinction on both levers (A1- and A2-), followed by separate presentations of each outcome, after which the lever presses were recorded, but not rewarded. (B) Simulated predictions for the responding of both sham and lesioned animals in reinstatement. (C) Mean percentage of baseline responding (±1 SEM) during reinstatement. Neuron 2015 88, 1268-1280DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 mOFC Lesions Spare Contingency Degradation (A) For contingency degradation: the instrumental phase is identical to Figure 1A. During the degradation, one of the outcomes was delivered outside of the A-O contingency. The rats were then given a choice test, A1 versus A2. (B) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) for sham animals during contingency degradation. (C) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) for lesioned animals during contingency degradation. (D) Simulated predictions for the responding of sham and lesioned animals during the contingency degradation test. (E) Mean percentage of baseline responding (±1 SEM) of sham and lesioned animals during the contingency degradation test. Neuron 2015 88, 1268-1280DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 mOFC Lesions Impair the Ability to Identify States Defined by Inhibitory Action-Outcome Contingencies (A) Rats received Pavlovian pretraining, during which they were trained to associate two stimuli, S1 and S2, with pellet and sucrose outcomes (O1 and O2, counterbalanced). Stage 1 then consisted of inhibitory contingency training: each time A1 action was taken S1 was presented, but the expected outcome O1 was not (A1-S1-noO1), and A2-S2 similarly predicted the absence of O2 (A2-S2-noO2). In Stage 2, rats were trained on the opposing excitatory associations (A1-S1-O1 and A2-S2-O2). On test, following 15 min of extinction on both levers, both actions earned S1 (i.e., A1-S1 and A2-S1). This stimulus was congruent with the trained contingencies for action A1 and incongruent for A2. Responding according to excitatory contingencies should be directed to A1 (i.e., congruent > incongruent), but responding according to inhibitory contingencies should be directed away from A1 and toward A2 (i.e., incongruent > congruent). The counterbalanced test was carried out 24 hr later. (B) Representation of lesion placements showing each overlapping cytotoxic lesion. (C) Mean magazine entries per minute (±1 SEM) during Pavlovian pretraining. (D) Mean magazine entries per minute (±1 SEM) during Stage 1 Pavlovian conditioning (that alternated with lever press sessions). (E) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) during Stage 1 training of inhibitory contingencies. (F) Mean magazine entries per minute (±1 SEM) during Stage 1 training of inhibitory contingencies. (G) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) during Stage 2 training of excitatory contingencies. (H) Mean magazine entries per minute (±1 SEM) during Stage 2 training of excitatory contingencies. (I) Simulated test results for Stage 3. (J) Mean lever pressing per minute (±1 SEM) during the Stage 3 test. Neuron 2015 88, 1268-1280DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2015.10.044) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions