Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Advertisements

Germán Sumbre, Graziano Fiorito, Tamar Flash, Binyamin Hochner 
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages e6 (April 2018)
Volume 25, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Idiothetic Path Integration in the Fruit Fly Drosophila melanogaster
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Navigation: Whence Our Sense of Direction?
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
EEG Responses to Visual Landmarks in Flying Pigeons
Chimpanzees Trust Their Friends
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Huan Luo, Xing Tian, Kun Song, Ke Zhou, David Poeppel  Current Biology 
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Bennett Drew Ferris, Jonathan Green, Gaby Maimon  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Yukiyasu Kamitani, Frank Tong  Current Biology 
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
Volume 27, Issue 5, Pages (March 2017)
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Disruption of Perceptual Learning by a Brief Practice Break
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e4 (December 2017)
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages e3 (December 2017)
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Vision Guides Selection of Freeze or Flight Defense Strategies in Mice
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Masayuki Matsumoto, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages e6 (April 2018)
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Michael L. Walls, John E. Layne  Current Biology 
Computer Use Changes Generalization of Movement Learning
Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon 
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
Environmental Consistency Determines the Rate of Motor Adaptation
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Fruit-Catching Fish Tune Their Fast Starts to Compensate for Drift
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Volume 78, Issue 4, Pages (May 2013)
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Volume 29, Issue 5, Pages e4 (March 2019)
Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys
Volume 15, Issue 9, Pages (May 2005)
J. Andrew Pruszynski, Roland S. Johansson, J. Randall Flanagan 
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Presentation transcript:

Bats Use Path Integration Rather Than Acoustic Flow to Assess Flight Distance along Flyways  Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 23, Pages 3650-3657.e3 (December 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.012 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Bats Do Not Use Statistical Acoustic Flow for Estimating Traveled Distance (A) Top view schematic of the experimental setup. Note that the x and y axes have different scales. (B and C) Prediction of bats’ searching behavior if they used statistical acoustic flow (B) or other navigation strategies such as path integration (C) under the different testing conditions (different colors). Each rectangle depicts a top view of the corridor, with colored bars showing the predicted searching location in each condition. (D) Searching behavior (position over time) of two individual bats under different acoustic-flow conditions. Panels show examples of one trial in each condition (for two bats). Turning points in the corridor are depicted by black circles. (E) The center and width of the searching location of four bats under different flow conditions. The center was defined as the mean of all turning points, and the width was defined as the mean distance between two consecutive turning points (STAR Methods). The vertical gray dotted line marks the original platform position at 20 m in all panels. See also Figure S1 and Tables S1 and S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 3650-3657.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.012) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Bats Use Path Integration for Estimating Traveled Distance (A–C) The experimental setup (A), predictions (B), and results (C) of three bats in the paradigm in which all poles were removed from the corridor. Note that the x and y axes have different scales. Green lines in (B) depict the prediction for the path-integration strategy in the corridor without poles. (D–F) The experimental setup (D), predictions (E), and results (F) of four bats in the paradigm in which the corridor was shifted by 10 m. The circles and arrows show the bats’ release points for the experiments with the original (black) and shifted corridor (green). Green lines in (E) depict the prediction for the path-integration strategy in the shifted corridor. In both (C) and (F), the black bars depict the same control trials as in Figure 1, which serve as a reference for where the bat learned the position of the platform. The center of the searching location was defined as the mean of all turning points, and the width was defined as the mean distance between two consecutive turning points (STAR Methods). The vertical gray dotted line marks the original platform position at 20 m in all panels. See also Table S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 3650-3657.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.012) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Bats Overestimate Target Location When Flying Faster (A) Searching location as a function of flight speed for three bats with additional weight (circles) and four bats without additional weight (asterisks). The mean speed in each trial was calculated by taking the mean of the momentary speed (STAR Methods). Black dashed line represents the mean ± SEM of all four bats. (B) Points of deceleration as a function of flight speed for three bats with additional weight (circles) and four bats without additional weight (asterisks). Black dashed line represents the mean ± SEM of all four bats. (C) Prediction of bats’ searching and deceleration locations when comparing the 50% slowest flights (black) and 50% fastest flights (gray). The width of the turning is predicted to be larger in the fast flights because deceleration takes more time when flying faster. (D) Deceleration and searching locations (mean ± turning width) of four bats are shown (deceleration location: t test, p = 0.03 for the group, and permutation t test, p < 0.02 for three individuals; p = 0.13 for the bat that died and did not complete the experiment; searching location: t test, p = 0.03 for the group, and permutation t test, p < 0.05 for all four individuals). Colors are the same as in (C); solid lines indicate searching locations, and dashed lines indicate deceleration locations. The vertical gray dotted line marks the original platform position at 20 m. Only the deceleration points toward the end of the corridor were used in this figure (as averaging the deceleration points in both directions would nullify the effect). Thus, the deceleration points appear after the mean searching points but before the end of the searching segment. The same pattern was obtained for the deceleration points in the opposite direction—there was a significant correlation between speed and deceleration in all bats (Pearson correlation: p < 0.05, R > 0.46 in all four bats). All bats decelerated later in this direction too, although two bats were only nearly significant (t test: p = 0.06 for the group; permutation t test: p < 0.05 for two individuals, p = 0.08 for the other two). See also Table S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 3650-3657.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.012) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Bats Underestimated the Location of the Target in Wind and Used Path Integration over 70 m (A) Two examples of flight trajectories in wind (same presentation as in Figure 1). Note how the turning positions gradually drift toward the beginning of the corridor—the opposite of the typical behavior. Arrows show the direction of the wind. The vertical gray dotted line marks the original platform position at 20 m. (B) The center and width of the searching location of four bats in the wind (gray) and the control (black) conditions. Arrows show the direction of the wind. The vertical gray dotted line marks the original platform position at 20 m. (C) The mean searching location of four individual bats as a function of accumulated flight distance is shown for the control condition (poles every 150 cm with no platform). See Figure S3 for the other conditions. Two cases are shown: the dashed line indicates when the bat came within the detection range of the start or end tulle walls, and the solid line indicates when the bat did not encounter the tulle walls. The mean ± SEM of all four bats is shown. To ascertain that the bats could sense the end tulle walls, we ran the same analysis but this time assumed a 5 m sensing range, and we got the same results of a gradually shifting searching location. See also Figure S3 and Table S2. Current Biology 2017 27, 3650-3657.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.012) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions