Andreia Santos, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg, Christine Deruelle 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The nature of Drosophila melanogaster
Advertisements

Human sleep and cortical reactivity are influenced by lunar phase
Volume 23, Issue 16, Pages R673-R676 (August 2013)
Can autistic children predict behavior by social stereotypes?
Mitosis: New Roles for Myosin-X and Actin at the Spindle
Turquoise killifish Current Biology
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Neuroscience: Impaired Decision-Making in Parkinson’s Disease
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Sexual Selection: Roles Evolving
Volume 21, Issue 20, Pages R837-R838 (October 2011)
Life History: The Energy-Efficient Orangutan
Volume 20, Issue 8, Pages R346-R349 (April 2010)
The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation
Infant cognition Current Biology
Female-led infanticide in wild chimpanzees
Synthetic Biology: Modulating the MAP Kinase Module
Volume 18, Issue 12, Pages R504-R505 (June 2008)
Behavior: Warriors Shaking Hands
Alternative Strategies: The Evolution of Switch Points
The neuroethics of non-invasive brain stimulation
Sexual Conflict: The Battle of the Sexes Reversed
Strepsiptera Current Biology
Joyce F. Benenson, Richard W. Wrangham  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Asymmetric tail-wagging responses by dogs to different emotive stimuli
Plant vacuoles Current Biology
Life History Evolution: What Does a Menopausal Killer Whale Do?
Sex Determination: Time for Meiosis? The Gonad Decides
Volume 20, Issue 4, Pages R136-R140 (February 2010)
Road crossing in chimpanzees: A risky business
Elephant cognition Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 11, Pages R453-R455 (June 2008)
Marine microplastics Current Biology
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Evolution: Mirror, Mirror in the Pond
Volume 25, Issue 19, Pages R815-R817 (October 2015)
Fifty years of illumination about the natural levels of adaptation
Sex ratios and social evolution
Volume 16, Issue 21, Pages R906-R910 (November 2006)
Figs and fig wasps Current Biology
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
It’s all about the constraints
Volume 23, Issue 7, Pages R273-R276 (April 2013)
Steven K. Schwartz, William E. Wagner, Eileen A. Hebets 
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Evidence of episodic-like memory in cuttlefish
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 20, Issue 14, Pages R590-R591 (July 2010)
Volume 25, Issue 10, Pages R394-R397 (May 2015)
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages R784-R785 (September 2012)
Early Life: Embracing the RNA World
FOXO transcription factors
Evolution: Lending a Helping Hand in Sperm Competition?
Hermaphroditic Sex Allocation Evolves When Mating Opportunities Change
Small RNAs: How Seeds Remember To Obey Their Mother
Rank influences human sex differences in dyadic cooperation
Anemonefishes Current Biology
Plant Development: Lessons from Getting It Twisted
Volume 28, Issue 8, Pages R350-R351 (April 2018)
Sensory Evolution: Trouble in the Cherry Orchard
Vision: Attending the Invisible
Matthew W. Hahn, Gregory C. Lanzaro  Current Biology 
Volume 20, Issue 8, Pages R346-R349 (April 2010)
Mates with Benefits: When and How Sexual Cannibalism Is Adaptive
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Reproductive Evolution: Symptom of a Selfing Syndrome
Volume 24, Issue 11, Pages R508-R510 (June 2014)
Presentation transcript:

Absence of racial, but not gender, stereotyping in Williams syndrome children  Andreia Santos, Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg, Christine Deruelle  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 7, Pages R307-R308 (April 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.009 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Lack of racial stereotyping by WS children. (A) Examples of racial-attitude (top) and sex-role (bottom) items. Racial attitude items: the two figures in each picture were identical except for the skin-colour difference — pinkish-tan versus medium brown. Figures of both sexes were employed, and a variety of ages were represented. The figures were drawn in a variety of sitting, standing, and walking positions, with the pictures being otherwise generally ambiguous as to any activities in which the persons represented might be engaged. Each picture was accompanied by a story containing one of six positive evaluative adjectives (such as kind, pretty, smart) or one of six negative evaluative adjectives (such as, bad, ugly, stupid), with the child being asked which of the two persons was the one described in the story. Sex role items: each of the sex role items displayed a male and female figure of the same general age, and of the same race (half of the pictures represented Caucasians; half, non-Caucasians). Sex role items assessed the child's knowledge of typical sex-stereotyped behaviours, and provided a control measure of general conceptual development. (B) Contrast of percentage-transformed responses in the in-group (sex-role) and the out-group (racial-attitude) conditions, 50% indicating no bias. Children with WS and control children showed a similar stereotype-consistent bias on the sex-role condition. Yet, on the racial-attitude condition, children with WS responses were significantly less stereotype-consistent than that of controls (Group x Condition interaction: F (1,38) = 7.93; p = 0.007), and showed no evidence for bias (X2 (1,19) = 28.0, p > 0.05). Current Biology 2010 20, R307-R308DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.009) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions