Takashi Sato, Aditya Murthy, Kirk G. Thompson, Jeffrey D. Schall 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Advertisements

Volume 53, Issue 1, Pages 9-16 (January 2007)
Volume 60, Issue 4, Pages (November 2008)
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Gijsbert Stoet, Lawrence H Snyder  Neuron 
Volume 51, Issue 6, Pages (September 2006)
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Ji Dai, Daniel I. Brooks, David L. Sheinberg  Current Biology 
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Reaches to Sounds Encoded in an Eye-Centered Reference Frame
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Goal-Related Activity in V4 during Free Viewing Visual Search
Attention-Induced Variance and Noise Correlation Reduction in Macaque V1 Is Mediated by NMDA Receptors  Jose L. Herrero, Marc A. Gieselmann, Mehdi Sanayei,
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Neural Correlates of Knowledge: Stable Representation of Stimulus Associations across Variations in Behavioral Performance  Adam Messinger, Larry R. Squire,
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Activity in Posterior Parietal Cortex Is Correlated with the Relative Subjective Desirability of Action  Michael C. Dorris, Paul W. Glimcher  Neuron 
Responses of Collicular Fixation Neurons to Gaze Shift Perturbations in Head- Unrestrained Monkey Reveal Gaze Feedback Control  Woo Young Choi, Daniel.
Volume 95, Issue 1, Pages e3 (July 2017)
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
A Dedicated Binding Mechanism for the Visual Control of Movement
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Liu D. Liu, Christopher C. Pack  Neuron 
Neural Correlates of Reaching Decisions in Dorsal Premotor Cortex: Specification of Multiple Direction Choices and Final Selection of Action  Paul Cisek,
Differences between Neural Activity in Prefrontal Cortex and Striatum during Learning of Novel Abstract Categories  Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Action Selection and Action Value in Frontal-Striatal Circuits
Attention Increases Sensitivity of V4 Neurons
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Brian D Corneil, Etienne Olivier, Douglas P Munoz  Neuron 
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Feature-based attention in visual cortex
Eye Movement Preparation Modulates Neuronal Responses in Area V4 When Dissociated from Attentional Demands  Nicholas A. Steinmetz, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff
Volume 29, Issue 2, Pages (February 2001)
Ryo Sasaki, Takanori Uka  Neuron  Volume 62, Issue 1, Pages (April 2009)
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Volume 54, Issue 2, Pages (April 2007)
Georgia G. Gregoriou, Stephen J. Gotts, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Volume 33, Issue 3, Pages (January 2002)
Hyoung F. Kim, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Monitoring and Control of Action by the Frontal Lobes
Short-Term Memory for Figure-Ground Organization in the Visual Cortex
Prefrontal Neurons Coding Suppression of Specific Saccades
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Hyoung F. Kim, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Phase Locking of Single Neuron Activity to Theta Oscillations during Working Memory in Monkey Extrastriate Visual Cortex  Han Lee, Gregory V. Simpson,
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Visuomotor Origins of Covert Spatial Attention
Biased Associative Representations in Parietal Cortex
Gijsbert Stoet, Lawrence H Snyder  Neuron 
John B Reppas, W.Martin Usrey, R.Clay Reid  Neuron 
Attention, Learning, and the Value of Information
The Postsaccadic Unreliability of Gain Fields Renders It Unlikely that the Motor System Can Use Them to Calculate Target Position in Space  Benjamin Y.
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Matthew I Leon, Michael N Shadlen  Neuron 
Dopamine Neurons Can Represent Context-Dependent Prediction Error
Prefrontal Control of Visual Distraction
Presentation transcript:

Search Efficiency but Not Response Interference Affects Visual Selection in Frontal Eye Field  Takashi Sato, Aditya Murthy, Kirk G. Thompson, Jeffrey D. Schall  Neuron  Volume 30, Issue 2, Pages 583-591 (May 2001) DOI: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X

Figure 1 Visual Search Tasks Each trial began with the presentation of a fixation point. After fixation for a variable interval the fixation point was removed and an eight-element circular search array with a single oddball target appeared. (A) Efficient color search with green target and red distractors. (B) Less efficient color search with green target and yellow-green distractors. (C) Motion search. Each stimulus was a circular aperture of randomly positioned dots. The direction of motion was either left or right, that of the target being the opposite from that of distractors. Search efficiency was manipulated by changing the proportion of dots moving coherently. (D) Search step task. On “No Step” trials, monkeys were rewarded for shifting gaze to the target. On “Target Step” trials, the target swapped positions with a distractor after a short “Step Delay,” and monkeys were rewarded for shifting gaze to the new target Neuron 2001 30, 583-591DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X)

Figure 2 Alternative Hypotheses about How the Time of Target Discrimination (TDT) and Reaction Time (RT) Vary with Search Efficiency or Response Interference Top trace shows the time of search array presentation with representative eye position traces for blocks of trials with short (RTS) and long (RTL) reaction times. Reaction time is subdivided into two intervals, one before TDT (thick line) and the other after TDT (thin line). Lower panels show the relation between TDT and the median reaction time for the two blocks of trials. If the increase in TDT accounts for the increase in reaction time due to the behavior manipulation, the fraction of the change in reaction time accounted for by the change in TDT (i.e., [TDTL−TDTS]/ [RTL−RTS]) should approach 100% (left). If TDT does not change and, therefore, does not account for the increase in reaction time due to the behavior manipulation, then the fraction of the change in reaction time accounted for by the change in TDT should be close to 0% (right) Neuron 2001 30, 583-591DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X)

Figure 3 Effect of Search Efficiency on Target Discrimination Time course of target discrimination by an FEF visual cell during efficient visual search (A) and during less efficient search (B) for trials with shortest (top) and longest reaction times (bottom). Left panels plot average discharge rate when the target fell in the neuron's receptive field (thick line) and when only distractors fell in the neuron's receptive field (thin line). Target and distractor features are indicated. The solid bar shown on the abscissa marks the range of reaction times. Dotted line shows the time of target discrimination (TDT). Right, ROC area as a function of time. The target discrimination time (TDT) is defined as the instant when the ROC area reaches 0.75. (C) Target discrimination time versus median reaction time for groups of trials with short, intermediate, and long reaction times. The dashed line plots the unity relation. (D) Activity during memory guided saccade trials aligned on the stimulus presentation (left) and on saccade initiation (right). The neuron was visually responsive with little sustained delay activity and no movement related modulation Neuron 2001 30, 583-591DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X)

Figure 4 Effect of Search Efficiency and Response Interference on Target Discrimination Time Relation between the time of target discrimination and reaction time in the less efficient search task (A and B) and the search step response interference task (C and D). (A) and (C) Each line connects values of TDT as a function of the median reaction time for the most extreme reaction time groups in which the neural activity discriminated the target from the distractors. (B) and (D) Distribution of the percentage of the change in reaction time accounted for by the change of the time of target discrimination for single FEF neurons Neuron 2001 30, 583-591DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X)

Figure 5 Time Course of Target Discrimination of an FEF Visual Cell in the Response Interference Task Monkeys shifted gaze to the target in a pop-out array that supported efficient search, but their reaction times were elevated because on some trials the target might change location. The increase in reaction time was not accompanied by an increase in target discrimination time. Conventions as in Figure 3 Neuron 2001 30, 583-591DOI: (10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00304-X)