Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages e7 (September 2017)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Advertisements

Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Flying Drosophila Orient to Sky Polarization
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
A Sparse Object Coding Scheme in Area V4
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Joshua P. Bassett, Thomas J. Wills, Francesca Cacucci  Current Biology 
How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Volume 22, Issue 13, Pages (July 2012)
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Enhanced Muscle Afferent Signals during Motor Learning in Humans
Volume 96, Issue 5, Pages e4 (December 2017)
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State
Yuki Hara, Akatsuki Kimura  Current Biology 
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Nathan F. Putman, Katherine L. Mansfield  Current Biology 
Archerfish Actively Control the Hydrodynamics of Their Jets
The Structure and Timescales of Heat Perception in Larval Zebrafish
A Switching Observer for Human Perceptual Estimation
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e7 (November 2017)
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
A Novel Form of Stereo Vision in the Praying Mantis
A Novel Form of Stereo Vision in the Praying Mantis
Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking Activity in Early Visual Cortex
Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages (July 2015)
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Chimeric Synergy in Natural Social Groups of a Cooperative Microbe
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 78, Issue 5, Pages (June 2013)
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Volume 23, Issue 17, Pages R709-R711 (September 2013)
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
Martin Eimer, Anna Grubert  Current Biology 
Origin and Function of Tuning Diversity in Macaque Visual Cortex
A Scalable Population Code for Time in the Striatum
Segregation of Object and Background Motion in Visual Area MT
Guilhem Ibos, David J. Freedman  Neuron 
Asif A. Ghazanfar, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, W. Tecumseh Fitch 
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Raghav Rajan, Allison J. Doupe  Current Biology 
Repeating Spatial Activations in Human Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Dynamic Shape Synthesis in Posterior Inferotemporal Cortex
Gaze and the Control of Foot Placement When Walking in Natural Terrain
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Rapid Spatial Learning Controls Instinctive Defensive Behavior in Mice
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e7 (November 2017)
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Honghui Zhang, Andrew J. Watrous, Ansh Patel, Joshua Jacobs  Neuron 
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Modeling Endoplasmic Reticulum Network Maintenance in a Plant Cell
Volume 29, Issue 5, Pages e4 (March 2019)
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Incidental Processing of Biological Motion
Collective Motion and Cannibalism in Locust Migratory Bands
The Geography of Ecological Niche Evolution in Mammals
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Metacognitive Failure as a Feature of Those Holding Radical Beliefs
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
George D. Dickinson, Ian Parker  Biophysical Journal 
Inverse Control of Turning Behavior by Dopamine D1 Receptor Signaling in Columnar and Ring Neurons of the Central Complex in Drosophila  Benjamin Kottler,
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Presentation transcript:

Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages 2862-2868.e7 (September 2017) Consistent Individual Differences Drive Collective Behavior and Group Functioning of Schooling Fish  Jolle W. Jolles, Neeltje J. Boogert, Vivek H. Sridhar, Iain D. Couzin, Andrea Manica  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages 2862-2868.e7 (September 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004 Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Group Shoaling Experiments (A–C) Schematics of (A) the free-schooling context, (B) the open foraging context with patches of food, and (C) the semi-covered foraging context with patches of food and plant cover. Schematics show tracking segments of one randomly selected group, with colors corresponding to the individual fish. Triangles point in the direction of motion. (D) Graphic illustrating key spatial and movement characteristics with arrows depicting movement vectors. For the individual assays, see Figure S1. Current Biology 2017 27, 2862-2868.e7DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Effect of Social Proximity Tendency on Spatial Positioning and Leadership (A) Fish nearest neighbor distance in groups as a function of their social proximity tendency, shown in five equally sized categories (mean ± 2 SEM; n = 120 fish). (B) Proportion of time fish occupied the most central to the most peripheral position in the group, calculated for each frame and averaged per individual across all frames (mean ± 2 SEM). (C) Density plot of the proportion of time individuals spent in front of the group center for the full 30 min trial. (D) Visualization of a leadership network in terms of propagation of speeding changes of one randomly selected group. Numbers indicate the average temporal delay in seconds and arrows point in the direction of propagation; see Figure S3. For plots (B) and (C), individuals were evenly distributed into three categories, with the intermediate category not shown for clarity. All data were analyzed as a continuous variable. See also Figures S2 and S4 for model simulations. Current Biology 2017 27, 2862-2868.e7DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Group Structure and Movement Dynamics in Relation to Group Mean Social Proximity Tendency (A–C) Heatmaps showing the distribution and link between the three key components of collective motion for groups with a low mean social proximity tendency (n = 13) relative to groups with a high mean social proximity tendency (n = 12). Groups with a relatively high social proximity tendency were more likely to be found in the bluer regions of the plots, whereas groups with relatively low social proximity tendency were more likely to be found in the redder regions of the plots. Group speed depicts the mean median swimming speed of the individuals in a group and is qualitatively similar to the speed of the group centroid. Plots are based on frame-by-frame data at time steps of 1/24th s, with groups evenly allocated to two categories based on their mean social proximity tendency. Units are in mean body length (BL; 40.6 mm), and contours represent iso-levels in percentage of the highest bin for all groups combined; see Figure S2. (D) Proportion of time groups were schooling, characterized based on the raw distributions of group speed, cohesion, and polarization (see STAR Methods). Solid gray line and dashed gray lines indicate a linear fit to the data with 95% confidence intervals. Current Biology 2017 27, 2862-2868.e7DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Effects of Individual Social Proximity and Exploratory Tendencies on Group Foraging Dynamics (A) Total number of foraging areas discovered during the open foraging context trials (out of 295 discoveries). (B) Inverted survival plot with confidence intervals of fish's likelihood to feed in the open and semi-covered foraging context. (C) Boxplots depicting total time spent out of plant cover alone in the semi-covered foraging context when food was still available. (D) Density plot of the mean number of food items eaten per trial across both foraging contexts. For plots (A)–(D), individual tendencies were evenly distributed into low, medium, and high categories (n = 42, n = 42, and n = 41 fish, respectively), with the intermediate category not shown for clarity. (E) Group foraging speed in the open (top) and semi-covered foraging cover context (bottom) in terms of the latency to consume each food item (15 provided per trial). The plot shows latencies averaged across trials for each group, and groups split into four categories based on their mean exploration and social proximity tendencies (low-low, low-high, high-low, and high-high: n = 5, 8, 8, and 4, respectively). (F) Surface plot of the mean number of food items eaten (log transformed) in the open foraging context (points indicate individual fish), based on a generalized linear mixed model (GLMM) fit to the data, cropped to 90% to show the effect excluding fish with the most extreme tendencies (n = 12 fish). Relative social proximity tendency is shown inverted such that faster fish are on the right and slower fish on the left, directly comparable with the model simulations of speed (see Figure S4). Current Biology 2017 27, 2862-2868.e7DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.004) Copyright © 2017 The Author(s) Terms and Conditions