Volume 84, Issue 2, Pages (October 2014)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Rapid Encoding of New Memories by Individual Neurons in the Human Brain Matias J. Ison, Rodrigo Quian Quiroga, Itzhak Fried Neuron Volume 87, Issue 1,
Advertisements

Soyoun Kim, Jaewon Hwang, Daeyeol Lee  Neuron 
Heterogeneous Coding of Temporally Discounted Values in the Dorsal and Ventral Striatum during Intertemporal Choice  Xinying Cai, Soyoun Kim, Daeyeol.
Risk-Responsive Orbitofrontal Neurons Track Acquired Salience
Volume 61, Issue 5, Pages (March 2009)
Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Volume 27, Issue 7, Pages (April 2017)
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Ranulfo Romo, Adrián Hernández, Antonio Zainos  Neuron 
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Rachel Ludmer, Yadin Dudai, Nava Rubin  Neuron 
Rhythmic Working Memory Activation in the Human Hippocampus
Volume 64, Issue 3, Pages (November 2009)
Internally Generated Preactivation of Single Neurons in Human Medial Frontal Cortex Predicts Volition  Itzhak Fried, Roy Mukamel, Gabriel Kreiman  Neuron 
Volume 64, Issue 4, Pages (November 2009)
Volume 83, Issue 3, Pages (August 2014)
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Daphna Shohamy, Anthony D. Wagner  Neuron 
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
Volume 84, Issue 1, Pages (October 2014)
Volume 86, Issue 6, Pages (June 2015)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Perirhinal-Hippocampal Connectivity during Reactivation Is a Marker for Object-Based Memory Consolidation  Kaia L. Vilberg, Lila Davachi  Neuron  Volume.
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Matias J. Ison, Rodrigo Quian Quiroga, Itzhak Fried  Neuron 
Volume 74, Issue 5, Pages (June 2012)
Theta-Coupled Periodic Replay in Working Memory
An Optimal Decision Population Code that Accounts for Correlated Variability Unambiguously Predicts a Subject’s Choice  Federico Carnevale, Victor de Lafuente,
Leila Reddy, Simon J. Thorpe  Neuron 
Inducing Gamma Oscillations and Precise Spike Synchrony by Operant Conditioning via Brain-Machine Interface  Ben Engelhard, Nofar Ozeri, Zvi Israel, Hagai.
Rodrigo Quian Quiroga, Alexander Kraskov, Christof Koch, Itzhak Fried 
Single Units in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex with Anxiety-Related Firing Patterns Are Preferentially Influenced by Ventral Hippocampal Activity  Avishek.
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
Volume 49, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Adaptation without Plasticity
Volume 90, Issue 1, Pages (April 2016)
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Multiple Timescales of Memory in Lateral Habenula and Dopamine Neurons
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Medial Axis Shape Coding in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex
Volume 63, Issue 5, Pages (September 2009)
Volume 28, Issue 9, Pages e4 (May 2018)
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Masayuki Matsumoto, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
Volume 95, Issue 5, Pages e5 (August 2017)
James M. Jeanne, Tatyana O. Sharpee, Timothy Q. Gentner  Neuron 
Rei Akaishi, Kazumasa Umeda, Asako Nagase, Katsuyuki Sakai  Neuron 
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
Contributions of Orbitofrontal and Lateral Prefrontal Cortices to Economic Choice and the Good-to-Action Transformation  Xinying Cai, Camillo Padoa-Schioppa 
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
Adaptation without Plasticity
Ethan S. Bromberg-Martin, Okihide Hikosaka  Neuron 
New and Distinct Hippocampal Place Codes Are Generated in a New Environment during Septal Inactivation  Mark P. Brandon, Julie Koenig, Jill K. Leutgeb,
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Michael J. Frank, Brion S. Woroch, Tim Curran  Neuron 
Arielle Tambini, Nicholas Ketz, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
Repeating Spatial Activations in Human Entorhinal Cortex
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Christoph Kayser, Nikos K. Logothetis, Stefano Panzeri  Current Biology 
Similarity Breeds Proximity: Pattern Similarity within and across Contexts Is Related to Later Mnemonic Judgments of Temporal Proximity  Youssef Ezzyat,
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Contributions of Orbitofrontal and Lateral Prefrontal Cortices to Economic Choice and the Good-to-Action Transformation  Xinying Cai, Camillo Padoa-Schioppa 
Volume 27, Issue 6, Pages (March 2017)
Spatiotemporal Neural Pattern Similarity Supports Episodic Memory
Presentation transcript:

Volume 84, Issue 2, Pages 363-369 (October 2014) Single-Cell Responses to Face Adaptation in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe  Rodrigo Quian Quiroga, Alexander Kraskov, Florian Mormann, Itzhak Fried, Christof Koch  Neuron  Volume 84, Issue 2, Pages 363-369 (October 2014) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.006 Copyright © 2014 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Behavioral Results (A) Adaptation paradigm. The perception of an ambiguous morphed image (A/B) was biased by the previous presentation of one of the pictures used to generate the morphing (picture A or picture B). The task of the subjects was to respond whether they recognized the ambiguous picture as A or B (here, presidents Bill Clinton and George Bush). (B) Mean percentage of trials in which subjects recognized the ambiguous image as B, when previously adapted to picture A (blue) or B (red). For comparison, the responses to the nonambiguous picture presentations (100% A and 100% B, likewise preceded by the adaptors) are also shown. (C) Same as (B) but separating between the 1–1.5 s and the 4 s presentation of the adaptors. The longer presentation of the adaptors led to a larger perceptual bias, namely the tendency to recognize the morphed picture as B when adapted to A (and vice versa). Error bars denote SEM. Neuron 2014 84, 363-369DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.006) Copyright © 2014 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Single Neuron Exemplary Responses Responses of a single unit in the hippocampus that fired strongly to the presentations of the picture of Whoopi Goldberg (100% B) but not to Bob Marley (100% A). The responses of the neuron to the pictures when used as adaptors (Adaptor A, Adaptor B) are also displayed. The unit had a larger response to the ambiguous pictures (M1, M2, and M3 pulled together; middle plots) when the subject recognized them as Goldberg (Decision B) compared to when he recognized them as Marley (Decision A). Based on the single-trial firing upon the presentation of the ambiguous pictures, a linear classifier could predict the subject’s decision significantly better than chance (p < 10−3; see Experimental Procedures). (See also Figures S1, S2, and S3 for additional examples.) Neuron 2014 84, 363-369DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.006) Copyright © 2014 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Population Results (A) Mean grand average responses for the three morphs used (M1, M2, and M3) and for the original (nonmorphed) images (A was the one image of the pair that was nonresponsive; while B was the responsive one). For each morph, note the significantly higher responses when the subject reported recognizing the image as B (p values for the average differences were obtained with Wilcoxon rank-sum tests). Error bars denote SEM. (B) Mean response strength for picture A, picture B, and the morphed pictures, separated according to the subjects’ report (Decided A or B). Note again the much higher response strength for the ambiguous pictures when recognized as B, which was similar to the response obtained when showing the original (nonmorphed) picture B. Likewise, the presentation of picture A gave a response that was statistically the same as the one obtained when showing picture A. Neuron 2014 84, 363-369DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.006) Copyright © 2014 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Average Instantaneous Firing Rates Grand average time courses of instantaneous firing rates for each condition: presentation of picture A (100% A), B (100% B), and ambiguous pictures recognized as A and B. Note the similar response pattern for picture B (responsive picture) and the ambiguous picture recognized as B. These responses were higher than the ones to the presentation of A (nonresponsive picture) or the ambiguous picture recognized as A. There were no significant differences in the latency of responses obtained in each condition (vertical lines). Shaded areas around mean values represent SEM. Neuron 2014 84, 363-369DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.006) Copyright © 2014 The Authors Terms and Conditions