The Sleeping Infant Brain Anticipates Development

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Neural Activity in the Central Complex of the Insect Brain Is Linked to Locomotor Changes John A. Bender, Alan J. Pollack, Roy E. Ritzmann Current Biology.
Advertisements

Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Calibrating color vision
Volume 26, Issue 16, Pages (August 2016)
Brain Responses in 4-Month-Old Infants Are Already Language Specific
Volume 28, Issue 6, Pages e4 (March 2018)
Decoding Wakefulness Levels from Typical fMRI Resting-State Data Reveals Reliable Drifts between Wakefulness and Sleep  Enzo Tagliazucchi, Helmut Laufs 
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Kevin Mann, Courtney L. Gallen, Thomas R. Clandinin  Current Biology 
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys
Thomas Andrillon, Sid Kouider, Trevor Agus, Daniel Pressnitzer 
Infant cognition Current Biology
Lexical Influences on Auditory Streaming
Huan Luo, Xing Tian, Kun Song, Ke Zhou, David Poeppel  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Memory Processing: Ripples in the Resting Brain
Giovanni M. Di Liberto, James A. O’Sullivan, Edmund C. Lalor 
Context Odor Presentation during Sleep Enhances Memory in Honeybees
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Louise Goupil, Sid Kouider  Current Biology 
Joyce F. Benenson, Richard W. Wrangham  Current Biology 
Volume 20, Issue 23, Pages (December 2010)
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Brain Responses in 4-Month-Old Infants Are Already Language Specific
Volume 26, Issue 13, Pages (July 2016)
Old-Growth Fishes Become Scarce under Fishing
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Tobias Staudigl, Simon Hanslmayr  Current Biology 
Chimeric Synergy in Natural Social Groups of a Cooperative Microbe
Volume 28, Issue 5, Pages e3 (March 2018)
Inducing Task-Relevant Responses to Speech in the Sleeping Brain
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
At Birth, Humans Associate “Few” with Left and “Many” with Right
Decoding the Yellow of a Gray Banana
Jason M. Cowell, Jean Decety  Current Biology 
Event-Based Prospective Memory in the Rat
Newborns' Cry Melody Is Shaped by Their Native Language
Intra- and Inter-Item Associations Doubly Dissociate the Electrophysiological Correlates of Familiarity and Recollection  Theodor Jäger, Axel Mecklinger,
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Elizabeth B. Klerman, Derk-Jan Dijk  Current Biology 
Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory  Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow,
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
How Dopamine Enhances an Optimism Bias in Humans
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
Training Attentional Control in Infancy
Federica Amici, Filippo Aureli, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
The Neural Basis of Language Development and Its Impairment
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Hermaphroditic Sex Allocation Evolves When Mating Opportunities Change
Knowledgeable Lemurs Become More Central in Social Networks
Medial Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Internally Driven Strategy Shifts
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
Michael L. Mack, Alison R. Preston, Bradley C. Love  Current Biology 
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Volume 18, Issue 20, Pages (October 2008)
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
Simon Hanslmayr, Jonas Matuschek, Marie-Christin Fellner 
Volume 26, Issue 16, Pages (August 2016)
Sleep Spindle Refractoriness Segregates Periods of Memory Reactivation
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Presentation transcript:

The Sleeping Infant Brain Anticipates Development Manuela Friedrich, Ines Wilhelm, Matthias Mölle, Jan Born, Angela D. Friederici  Current Biology  Volume 27, Issue 15, Pages 2374-2380.e3 (August 2017) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2017 27, 2374-2380.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Design (A) In the learning phase, half of the presented object-word pairs appeared in the consistent pairing condition, in which a word was paired once with each of eight novel, but similar, objects, allowing the learning of category-word pairings. The other half appeared in the inconsistent pairing condition, in which a word was repeated for the same number of times but paired once with each of eight dissimilar objects, such that stable pairings could not be learned. (B) In the memory test, each of the four novel objects of the categories were presented either as correct pairings with their labeling word of the learning phase or as incorrect pairings with a word previously taken to label another category. Current Biology 2017 27, 2374-2380.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Category-Word Pairing Effects during Learning and Memory Testing The infant ERPs in response to words paired with novel category exemplars. Following standard analyses of the N400 priming effect, we calculated all memory effects as the difference between unprimed and primed conditions (i.e., inconsistent − consistent and incorrect − correct); negativity is plotted upward. (A) No evidence for generalization during learning. (B) No evidence for generalization in the memory test of the wake group. (C) Late negativity (cluster involving CP5, T7, P7, P3, PZ, P4, C3, CZ, C4, and FC3) in the memory test of the short-nap group, indicating the presence of lower-developed perceptual-associative memory for the category-word pairings. (D) N400 cluster (involving PZ, CZ, P4, and C4) in the memory test of the long-nap group, indicating the presence of higher-developed lexical-semantic memory. Current Biology 2017 27, 2374-2380.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 The Time Course of Memory Formation during Sleep Stage 2 (A) Late negative memory effect in the short-N2 group (infants with N2 duration below or equal to the median of 9.5 min). (B) N400 memory effect in the long-N2 group (infants with N2 duration above the median of 9.5 min). (C) No evidence for a memory effect in infants, who stayed in N2 for 0.5–4 min. (D) Late negativity (t18 = 2.629, p = 0.017) in infants, who stayed in N2 for 4.5–9.5 min, indicating the presence of perceptually based associative memory. (E) Missing memory effect in infants, who stayed in N2 for 10–17.5 min, suggesting that memory reorganization was in progress. (F) N400 effect (t16 = −2.356, p = 0.032) in infants, who stayed in N2 for 18–35 min, attesting the establishment of lexical-semantic memory. See also Table S3 and S4. Current Biology 2017 27, 2374-2380.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 The Relation between Sleep Spindles and Lexical-Semantic Memory (A) EEG power during NREM sleep for frontal (mean of F3, FZ, and F4), central (mean of C3, CZ, and C4), and parietal (mean of P3, PZ, and P4) channels. (B) Averaged NREM sleep spindles for the short- and long-N2 groups. (C) Correlation between mean peak-to-peak amplitude of sleep spindles at the right central brain region (C4) during sleep stage 2 of the nap in the retention period and the N400 semantic generalization effect in the memory test of the long-N2 group (r = −0.488, p = 0.006). (D) Correlation between mean spindle RMS at C4 in N2 and the N400 memory effect of the long-N2 group (r = −0.526, p = 0.003). Note that due to the negative polarity of the N400 effect the correlations are also negative. See also Figure S1. Current Biology 2017 27, 2374-2380.e3DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.070) Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions