Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages (January 2007)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Adolescent Behavior and Dopamine Availability Are Uniquely Sensitive to Dietary Omega-3 Fatty Acid Deficiency  Corina O. Bondi, Ameer Y. Taha, Jody L.
Advertisements

Volume 86, Issue 5, Pages (June 2015)
Motor Protein KIF1A Is Essential for Hippocampal Synaptogenesis and Learning Enhancement in an Enriched Environment  Makoto Kondo, Yosuke Takei, Nobutaka.
Covalent Modification of DNA Regulates Memory Formation
Volume 79, Issue 1, Pages (July 2013)
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Volume 79, Issue 6, Pages (September 2013)
Volume 79, Issue 3, Pages (August 2013)
Hiroaki Norimoto, Yuji Ikegaya  Current Biology 
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Volume 77, Issue 5, Pages (March 2013)
Volume 21, Issue 2, Pages (August 1998)
Chenguang Zheng, Kevin Wood Bieri, Yi-Tse Hsiao, Laura Lee Colgin 
Volume 75, Issue 4, Pages (August 2012)
Masahiro Yasuda, Mark R. Mayford  Neuron 
Environmental Enrichment Rescues Binocular Matching of Orientation Preference in Mice that Have a Precocious Critical Period  Bor-Shuen Wang, Liang Feng,
Volume 55, Issue 5, Pages (September 2007)
Volume 8, Issue 1, Pages (January 2017)
Hill-Aina Steffenach, Menno Witter, May-Britt Moser, Edvard I. Moser 
Acetylcholine-secreting cells improve age-induced memory deficits
Volume 47, Issue 6, Pages (September 2005)
Volume 15, Issue 9, Pages (May 2016)
Volume 144, Issue 5, Pages (March 2011)
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 4, Issue 3, Pages (August 2013)
Leslie R. Whitaker, Mickael Degoulet, Hitoshi Morikawa  Neuron 
Volume 49, Issue 5, Pages (March 2006)
Volume 23, Issue 3, Pages (February 2013)
NMDA Receptors in Dopaminergic Neurons Are Crucial for Habit Learning
Dynamics of Retrieval Strategies for Remote Memories
Volume 50, Issue 5, Pages (June 2006)
Volume 38, Issue 5, Pages (June 2003)
Volume 93, Issue 6, Pages e6 (March 2017)
Volume 11, Issue 2, Pages (April 2015)
Single sevoflurane exposure decreases neuronal nitric oxide synthase levels in the hippocampus of developing rats  X. Feng, J.J. Liu, X. Zhou, F.H. Song,
Volume 45, Issue 5, Pages (March 2005)
Gating of Fear in Prelimbic Cortex by Hippocampal and Amygdala Inputs
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Representation of Geometric Borders in the Developing Rat
Volume 41, Issue 1, Pages (January 2004)
Amygdalar MicroRNA-15a Is Essential for Coping with Chronic Stress
μ-Opioid Receptor and CREB Activation Are Required for Nicotine Reward
Volume 71, Issue 5, Pages (September 2011)
Volume 55, Issue 2, Pages (July 2007)
Volume 43, Issue 1, Pages (July 2004)
Volume 3, Issue 3, Pages (September 2008)
Volume 86, Issue 5, Pages (June 2015)
Volume 94, Issue 2, Pages e4 (April 2017)
Volume 50, Issue 3, Pages (May 2006)
Volume 23, Issue 2, Pages (January 2013)
Stéphanie Trouche, Jennifer M. Sasaki, Tiffany Tu, Leon G. Reijmers 
Volume 16, Issue 5, Pages (August 2016)
Volume 23, Issue 6, Pages (March 2013)
New and Distinct Hippocampal Place Codes Are Generated in a New Environment during Septal Inactivation  Mark P. Brandon, Julie Koenig, Jill K. Leutgeb,
Volume 86, Issue 6, Pages (June 2015)
Farah D. Lubin, J. David Sweatt  Neuron 
Volume 106, Issue 6, Pages (September 2001)
Volume 53, Issue 5, Pages (March 2007)
Attenuated Cold Sensitivity in TRPM8 Null Mice
Volume 129, Issue 1, Pages (April 2007)
Volume 93, Issue 3, Pages e5 (February 2017)
Volume 89, Issue 4, Pages (February 2016)
Volume 135, Issue 3, Pages (October 2008)
Volume 66, Issue 6, Pages (June 2010)
Volume 90, Issue 4, Pages (May 2016)
Volume 71, Issue 5, Pages (September 2011)
Volume 20, Issue 6, Pages (August 2017)
Spatial Representation along the Proximodistal Axis of CA1
Volume 47, Issue 6, Pages (September 2005)
Irreplaceability of Neuronal Ensembles after Memory Allocation
Presentation transcript:

Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages 261-277 (January 2007) Persistence of Long-Term Memory Storage Requires a Late Protein Synthesis- and BDNF- Dependent Phase in the Hippocampus  Pedro Bekinschtein, Martín Cammarota, Lionel Müller Igaz, Lia R.M. Bevilaqua, Iván Izquierdo, Jorge H. Medina  Neuron  Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages 261-277 (January 2007) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025 Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Cannulae Placements and Drug Infusion (A) Photomicrograph of a representative Nissl-stained coronal brain section showing the infusion cannula track terminating in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. (B) Schematic representations of rat brain sections at three rostrocaudal planes (−3.80, −4.30, and −4.80 from bregma) taken from the atlas of Paxinos and Watson (1986), showing, in stippling, the extension of the area reached by the infusions in the dorsal hippocampus. Reprinted from The Rat Brain in stereo taxic coordinates by Paxinos and Watson, pages 33, 35, and 37, Academic Press (1997), with permission from Elsevier. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Inhibition of Protein Synthesis in the Rat Hippocampus 12 hr after Training Hinders Persistence, but Not Formation, of Hippocampus-Dependent Memory (A) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Male Wistar rats (2.5 months old, 220–250 g) were bilaterally infused with vehicle (Veh) or anisomycin (Ani) (80 μg/side) into the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus, 15 min before or 3, 9, 12, and 24 hr after IA training. Data are expressed as the mean (±SEM) of training (TR, black bars) or test session step-down latency at 2 days (white bars) or 7 days (gray bars) after IA training. ∗p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01, and ∗∗∗p < 0.001, versus Veh group; two-tailed Student's t test, n = 10–12 per group. (B) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Independent groups of animals were infused with Veh (white bars) or Ani (gray bars) 12 hr after IA training and tested 2, 4, or 7 days posttraining. Data are expressed as in (A). ∗p < 0.05 versus the corresponding Veh group; two-tailed Student's t test, n = 10–12 per group. (C) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Animals injected in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus with Veh (open circles and white bars) or Ani (gray circles and gray bars) 12 hr after IA training were submitted 7 days later to the hidden platform version of the Morris Water Maze. The left panel shows mean escape latencies during the 5 days of acquisition of spatial learning. Data are presented in blocks of eight trials as the mean ± SD, p > 0.05 versus Veh in Bonferroni post hoc test after two-way ANOVA, n = 8 per group. The right panel shows the mean time spent in the target quadrant (TQ) during a 60 s probe test carried out in the absence of the escape platform 24 hr after the last training day. Data are presented in blocks of eight trials as the mean (±SD), p = 0.24, two-tailed Student's t test, n = 8 per group. (D) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Animals were infused with Veh or Ani (80 μg/side) into the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus, 15 min before or 12 hr after CFC training. Data are expressed as mean (±SEM) percentage of freezing before training (preshock, black bars) or during a memory retention test carried out 2 (white bars) or 7 (gray bars) days after CFC training.∗∗p < 0.01 and ∗∗∗p < 0.001, versus Veh group; two-tailed Student's t test, n = 8 per group. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Infusion of Anisomycin 12 hr after IA Training Does Not Affect Exploratory Behavior, Basal Locomotor Activity, or Anxiety, and Does Not Cause Lesion to the Hippocampus 7 Days Posttraining (A) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Representative photomicrographs showing Nissl-stained coronal brain sections of rats infused with Veh (left) or Ani (right) 12 hr after IA training and sacrificed 7 days later. There was no evidence of cell loss or gliosis. (B) Top panels show the number of rearings (left) and crossings (right) during a 5 min open field session for animals that had received Veh (open bars) or Ani (gray bars) in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus 12 hr after IA training, 2 or 7 days before. Data are expressed as mean (±SEM) number of crossings or rearings, p > 0.1 in a two-tailed Student's t test, n = 8 per group. Bottom panels show the total number of entries (left), the time spent in open arms (middle), and the number of entries into the open arms (right) during a 5 min plus maze session for rats that had received bilateral intra-CA1 infusion of Veh (white bars) or Ani (gray bars) 2 or 7 days before; p > 0.1 in a two-tailed Student's t test, n = 8 per group. The upper part of both panels show the schematic of the procedure used in each experiment. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Hippocampal BDNF Is Required at 12 hr Posttraining for Persistence, but Not Formation, of IA Memory (A) Time-restricted expression of BDNF was detected at 12 (middle), but not at 9 (left) or 24 (right), hr after IA training. The dorsal hippocampus was dissected out from naive rats (white bars) or IA trained rats (9, 12, or 24 hr posttraining, gray bars) and total homogenates were subjected to SDS-PAGE followed by western blot analysis with antibodies against BDNF or β-actin. (Top) Bars show normalized mean percentage levels respect to the naive (N) group. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM, ∗p < 0.05, trained (12 hr) versus naive, Student's t test, n = 6 per group. (B) (Left) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Right) Inhibitory avoidance learning-induced increase in BDNF expression around 12 hr posttraining is abolished by a preceeding intra-CA1 infusion of Ani. IA trained rats received bilateral intra-CA1 infusions of Veh (trained, Veh) or Ani (80 μg/side) (trained, Ani) 11.5 hr posttraining. Thirty minutes later animals were killed by decapitation and the dorsal hippocampus was dissected out and utilized to prepare a total homogenate on which western blot analysis for BDNF and β-actin content was performed. In addition, two control groups were evaluated: nontrained animals (naive, N group) and nontrained animals that were infused with Ani and sacrificed 30 min afterwards (naive, N Ani group). (Top) Bars show normalized mean percentage levels with respect to the control (N) group. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. ∗p < 0.05 in Newman-Keuls test after one-way ANOVA, n = 6 per group. (Bottom) Representative western blots showing BDNF and β-actin levels. One nanogram of human recombinant BDNF (hrBDNF) was loaded as control for antibody specificity (lane five). (C) (Left) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Right) Blockade of BDNF activity at 12 hr after training hinders persistence, but not formation, of IA memory. Animals were infused with Veh or BDNF-blocking antibody (0.5 μg/side; anti-BDNF) into the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus 15 min before or 9, 12, or 24 hr after training. Data are expressed as mean (±SEM) of training (TR, black bars) or test session step-down latency, 2 (white bars) or 7 (gray bars) days after IA training. ∗∗p < 0.01, versus Veh group; Student's t test, n = 10–12 per group. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Intra-Hippocampal Infusion of BDNF Antisense Oligonucleotide 10 hr after IA Training Blocks Memory Retention at 7, but Not at 2, Days Posttraining (A) Biotinylated antisense oligonucleotide (ASO) distribution and relative concentrations at different times after infusion (2 nmol/μl; 1 μl/side) in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus. Rats were injected and sacrificed 2 (left) or 24 (right) hr after infusion. By 2 hr, the ASO diffused throughout the dorsal hippocampus and slightly into the overlying cortex. After 24 hr, the ASO was cleared out from the hippocampus. (B) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Infusion of BDNF ASO in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus transiently decreases BDNF steady-state levels. Rats were injected with BDNF scrambled missense oligonucleotide (MSO) (white bars) or BDNF ASO (black bars) and sacrificed 2 (left), 6 (middle), or 24 (right) hr after infusion. Animals were killed by decapitation and the dorsal hippocampus was dissected out and homogenized to perform western blot analysis of BDNF content. (Top) Bars show normalized mean percentage levels with respect to animals injected with BDNF MSO. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. ∗∗p = 0.006, ASO versus MSO; Student's t test, n = 4 per group. (Bottom) Representative western blots showing BDNF and β-actin levels. (C) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) Intra-CA1 BDNF ASO infusion 10 hr after IA training prevented the learning-associated increase in BDNF protein 12 hr after training. Naive or IA trained rats received bilateral intra-CA1 infusions of BDNF MSO (white bars) or BDNF ASO (gray bars). Two hours later the animals were sacrificed and the dorsal hippocampus was dissected out and used to prepare total homogenates for western blot analysis of BDNF content. (Top) Bars show the normalized mean percentage levels with respect to the naive animals injected with MSO. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. ∗p < 0.05 in Newman-Keuls test after ANOVA, n = 5 per group. (Bottom) Representative western blots showing BDNF and β-actin levels. (D) (Top) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Bottom) BDNF ASO, but not BDNF MSO, infusion 10 hr after IA training hinders memory persistence at 7 days, but left memory intact 2 days, posttraining. Animals were infused with BDNF MSO (2 nmol; 1 μl/side) (white bars) or BDNF ASO (2 nmol; 1 μl/side) (gray bars) into the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus 10 hr after training. Data are expressed as mean (± SEM) of training (TR, black bars) or test session step-down latency, 2 or 7 days after IA training. ∗∗p < 0.01, ASO versus MSO at 7 days; Student's t test, n = 8–10 per group. (E) (Left) Schematic of the procedure used in this experiment. (Right) Intra-CA1 infusion of hrBDNF (0.25 μg/side), but not Veh, 12 hr after IA training rescued the BDNF ASO-induced memory deficit at 7 days posttraining. Data are expressed as mean (± SEM) of training (TR, black bars) or test session step-down latency 7 days after IA training. ∗∗p < 0.01, ASO+Veh versus MSO+Veh; ∗p < 0.05, ASO+hrBDNF versus MSO+Veh; n = 10, Newman-Keuls test after one-way ANOVA. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions

Figure 6 Intra-CA1 Infusion of Ani or Anti-BDNF 12 hr Posttraining Blocks the IA Learning-Induced Increase in c-Fos and Zif268 Levels that Occurs Late after Training (Top) IA trained animals were injected in the dorsal CA1 region with Veh (white bars) or Ani (80 μg/side) (gray bars) 12 hr after acquisition of IA and sacrificed 24 hr posttraining. The hippocampus was dissected out and homogenized to perform western blot analysis of (A) c-Fos and (B) Zif268 expression. Bars show normalized mean percentage levels with respect to the naive group. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. ∗p < 0.05, trained versus naive, one-way ANOVA, n = 6 per group. (Bottom) In an independent series of experiments, IA trained animals were injected with Veh (white bars) or anti-BDNF (0.5 μg/side) (gray bars) 12 hr after acquisition of IA and sacrificed 24 hr afterwards. The hippocampus was dissected out and homogenized to carry out western blot analysis of (C) c-Fos and (D) Zif268 expression. Bars show normalized mean percentage levels with respect to the naive group. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. ∗∗p < 0.01, ∗p < 0.05, trained versus naive, one-way ANOVA, n = 6 per group. The upper part of each panel shows the schematic of the procedure used in each experiment. Neuron 2007 53, 261-277DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.025) Copyright © 2007 Elsevier Inc. Terms and Conditions