Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages (July 2015)

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Dissociable Effects of Serotonin and Dopamine on the Valuation of Harm in Moral Decision Making Molly J. Crockett, Jenifer Z. Siegel, Zeb Kurth-Nelson,
Advertisements

Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Acute Stress Impairs Self-Control in Goal-Directed Choice by Altering Multiple Functional Connections within the Brain’s Decision Circuits  Silvia U.
Dopamine Modulates Egalitarian Behavior in Humans
How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects
Illusory Jitter Perceived at the Frequency of Alpha Oscillations
Volume 80, Issue 4, Pages (November 2013)
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Volume 88, Issue 3, Pages (November 2015)
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Volume 72, Issue 4, Pages (November 2011)
With Age Comes Representational Wisdom in Social Signals
Chenguang Zheng, Kevin Wood Bieri, Yi-Tse Hsiao, Laura Lee Colgin 
Dopamine Modulates Option Generation for Behavior
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
Christiane M Thiel, Karl J Friston, Raymond J Dolan  Neuron 
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Volume 17, Issue 21, Pages (November 2007)
Volume 85, Issue 2, Pages (January 2015)
Nils Kolling, Marco Wittmann, Matthew F.S. Rushworth  Neuron 
Dopamine Modulates Egalitarian Behavior in Humans
Volume 93, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
The Privileged Brain Representation of First Olfactory Associations
Hedging Your Bets by Learning Reward Correlations in the Human Brain
Dopamine Reward Prediction Error Responses Reflect Marginal Utility
Volume 82, Issue 5, Pages (June 2014)
Volume 26, Issue 13, Pages (July 2016)
Inactivation of Medial Frontal Cortex Changes Risk Preference
High Resilience of Seed Dispersal Webs Highlighted by the Experimental Removal of the Dominant Disperser  Sérgio Timóteo, Jaime Albino Ramos, Ian Phillip.
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Volume 74, Issue 3, Pages (May 2012)
A Neurocomputational Model of Altruistic Choice and Its Implications
Confidence Is the Bridge between Multi-stage Decisions
Fast Sequences of Non-spatial State Representations in Humans
Neural Predictors of Purchases
Neural Mechanisms Underlying Human Consensus Decision-Making
Volume 92, Issue 5, Pages (December 2016)
The Social Dominance Paradox
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Active Interpersonal Touch Gives Rise to the Social Softness Illusion
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Computer Use Changes Generalization of Movement Learning
Megan E. Speer, Jamil P. Bhanji, Mauricio R. Delgado  Neuron 
Functional MRI Reveals Compromised Neural Integrity of the Face Processing Network in Congenital Prosopagnosia  Galia Avidan, Marlene Behrmann  Current.
How Dopamine Enhances an Optimism Bias in Humans
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e3 (October 2017)
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e2 (September 2017)
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
David Badre, Bradley B. Doll, Nicole M. Long, Michael J. Frank  Neuron 
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Volume 28, Issue 7, Pages e5 (April 2018)
Acute Stress Impairs Self-Control in Goal-Directed Choice by Altering Multiple Functional Connections within the Brain’s Decision Circuits  Silvia U.
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Impaired Associative Learning with Food Rewards in Obese Women
J. Andrew Pruszynski, Roland S. Johansson, J. Randall Flanagan 
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Orbitofrontal Cortex Uses Distinct Codes for Different Choice Attributes in Decisions Motivated by Curiosity  Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden,
Sleep Spindle Refractoriness Segregates Periods of Memory Reactivation
Volume 28, Issue 19, Pages e8 (October 2018)
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Volume 28, Issue 18, Pages e3 (September 2018)
A Goal Direction Signal in the Human Entorhinal/Subicular Region
Presentation transcript:

Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages 1852-1859 (July 2015) Dissociable Effects of Serotonin and Dopamine on the Valuation of Harm in Moral Decision Making  Molly J. Crockett, Jenifer Z. Siegel, Zeb Kurth-Nelson, Olga T. Ousdal, Giles Story, Carolyn Frieband, Johanna M. Grosse-Rueskamp, Peter Dayan, Raymond J. Dolan  Current Biology  Volume 25, Issue 14, Pages 1852-1859 (July 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021 Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Design In each trial, deciders chose between less money and fewer shocks versus more money and more shocks. (A–D) The money was always for the decider, but in half the trials, the shocks were allocated to the decider (A and C), and, in the other half, the shocks were allocated to the receiver (B and D). In all trials, if the decider failed to press a key within 6 s, the highlighted default (top) option was registered; if the decider pressed the key, the alternative (bottom) option was highlighted and registered instead. In half the trials, the alternative option contained more money and shocks than the default (A and B), so action resulted in greater harm and profit. In the other half, the alternative option contained less money and shocks than the default (C and D), so inaction resulted in greater harm and profit. Current Biology 2015 25, 1852-1859DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Effects of Citalopram and Levodopa on Harm Aversion and Hyperaltruism in Study 1 and Study 2 (A) In study 1, Citalopram significantly increased harm aversion for self (κself) and others (κother) but did not affect the difference in harm aversion between self and others (i.e., hyperaltruism). (B) Sorted effect sizes of hyperaltruism (defined by taking the difference between κother and κself) across participants for the placebo and citalopram groups in study 1. Black bars indicate hyperaltruistic subjects, while white bars indicate selfish subjects. (C) In study 2, Levodopa did not affect harm aversion for self or others, but significantly decreased the difference in harm aversion between self and others. (D) Sorted effect sizes of hyperaltruism (κother − κself) across participants for the placebo and levodopa groups in study 2. ∗p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; n.s., not significant. Error bars represent SEM difference between κself and κother. Current Biology 2015 25, 1852-1859DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Predictions of Fitted Regression Models of the Interaction of Drug and Effective Dosage on Harm Aversion for Self and Others and Hyperaltruism (A) Citalopram increased harm aversion for self and others relative to placebo, more strongly for subjects with lower body weight (who thus received a higher effective dose). (B) Levodopa reduced hyperaltruism relative to placebo, more strongly for subjects with lower body weight (who thus received a higher effective dose). Current Biology 2015 25, 1852-1859DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Effects of Hyperaltruism and Drugs on Response Times (A and B) Hyperaltruism (κother − κself) was positively correlated with slower decisions for others relative to self in study 1 (A) and study 2 (B), as measured by the weight of a general linear model regressor indicating whether the outcome was for other (βother). (C) Levodopa reduced slowing associated with deciding for others, relative to self, but did not affect speeding related to value differences (|ΔV|). (D) Citalopram reduced speeding related to value differences but did not affect slowing associated with deciding for others, relative to self. ∗p < 0.05. Error bars represent SEM. a.u., arbitrary units. See also Table S3. Current Biology 2015 25, 1852-1859DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.021) Copyright © 2015 The Authors Terms and Conditions