Thomas S Collett, Paul Graham  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
David G. Hazlerigg, Francis J.P. Ebling, Jonathan D. Johnston 
Advertisements

Gemmata obscuriglobus
Matthew Collett, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Jean-Paul Noel, Mark Wallace, Randolph Blake  Current Biology 
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 14, Issue 6, Pages R221-R224 (March 2004)
Spatial Memory: Mice Quickly Learn a Safe Haven
Animal Vision: Rats Watch the Sky
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
Behavioural Biology: Fortune Favours Bold and Shy Personalities
Matthew Collett, Thomas S. Collett, Mandyam V. Srinivasan 
Mark J. Costello, Chhaya Chaudhary  Current Biology 
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Autodesk Professional User: AutoCAD
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Honeybee Vision: In Good Shape for Shape Recognition
Cell Biology: Microtubule Collisions to the Rescue
Chimpanzees Trust Their Friends
Volume 27, Issue 12, Pages R599-R600 (June 2017)
Volume 22, Issue 16, Pages R615-R618 (August 2012)
A new galloping gait in an insect
Tzvi Ganel, Eran Chajut, Daniel Algom  Current Biology 
Planarians Current Biology
Infant cognition Current Biology
Visual Perception: Lightness in a High-Dynamic-Range World
Visual Attention: Bottom-Up Versus Top-Down
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation
Animal Navigation: Salmon Track Magnetic Variation
RecA Current Biology Volume 17, Issue 11, Pages R395-R397 (June 2007)
Face Perception: Broken into Parts
Animal Evolution: Last Word on Sponges-First?
Volume 24, Issue 18, Pages R867-R871 (September 2014)
Elephant cognition Current Biology
Quantity Cognition: Numbers, Numerosity, Zero and Mathematics
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Volume 22, Issue 20, Pages (October 2012)
Homing Ants Get Confused When Nest Cues Are Also Route Cues
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
At Birth, Humans Associate “Few” with Left and “Many” with Right
Structure and function of the cerebral cortex
Flies see second-order motion
Investigating Physical Cognition in Rooks, Corvus frugilegus
Insect Vision: Controlling Actions through Optic Flow
Robert A. Harris, Paul Graham, Thomas S. Collett  Current Biology 
Place, space and memory cells
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Animal Cognition: An Insect's Sense of Time?
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Volume 25, Issue 10, Pages R394-R397 (May 2015)
Volume 23, Issue 24, Pages R1083-R1085 (December 2013)
Path Integration: Combining Optic Flow with Compass Orientation
Volume 7, Issue 6, Pages R147-R151 (June 2000)
Passive Transport Disrupts Grid Signals in the Parahippocampal Cortex
Small RNAs: How Seeds Remember To Obey Their Mother
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
Recurrent neuronal circuits in the neocortex
How Navigational Guidance Systems Are Combined in a Desert Ant
Incidental Processing of Biological Motion
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
David G. Hazlerigg, Francis J.P. Ebling, Jonathan D. Johnston 
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Animal navigation: Birds as geometers?
Calibration of vector navigation in desert ants
Circadian Biology: The Early Bird Catches the Morning Shift
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Animal Navigation: Path Integration, Visual Landmarks and Cognitive Maps  Thomas S Collett, Paul Graham  Current Biology  Volume 14, Issue 12, Pages R475-R477 (June 2004) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.013

Figure 1 Two path integration systems and their interactions with landmarks. Left: path integration positions, in Cartesian coordinates, relative to the origin. Animals associate average path integration coordinates experienced at a place, for example C, with their view of the world from C. Dotted paths indicate journeys of different lengths to C. A direct path between A and B is specified by the difference between the path integration coordinates of A and B. Right: animals store a graph of connected recognizable places; path integration is used to navigate along known vectors between those places. Current Biology 2004 14, R475-R477DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.013)

Figure 2 Homing directions after a visual fix. Left column: experimental arrangement. Right column: results. Top row: nest door in wall of arena. Bottom row: nest hole in floor. Hamsters are led on a short journey in the dark after the visual fix (dotted line). In both cases hamsters aim at the visually defined nest location (open rectangle or circle), rather than at the path integration defined location (filled rectangle or circle). The hamsters' paths are shown by the solid lines. Thus, path integration coordinates are reset by the visual fix. Current Biology 2004 14, R475-R477DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2004.06.013)