Face Perception Neuron

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
The Parahippocampal Place Area
Advertisements

Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Volume 61, Issue 5, Pages (March 2009)
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Volume 60, Issue 5, Pages (December 2008)
Long-Term Speeding in Perceptual Switches Mediated by Attention-Dependent Plasticity in Cortical Visual Processing  Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky 
Volume 73, Issue 3, Pages (February 2012)
Illusory Jitter Perceived at the Frequency of Alpha Oscillations
Coding of Cognitive Magnitude
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Daphna Shohamy, Anthony D. Wagner  Neuron 
Volume 35, Issue 4, Pages (August 2002)
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Rajeev D.S. Raizada, Russell A. Poldrack  Neuron 
Predicting Value of Pain and Analgesia: Nucleus Accumbens Response to Noxious Stimuli Changes in the Presence of Chronic Pain  Marwan N. Baliki, Paul.
Sheng Li, Stephen D. Mayhew, Zoe Kourtzi  Neuron 
Volume 74, Issue 5, Pages (June 2012)
Volume 53, Issue 6, Pages (March 2007)
Caspar M. Schwiedrzik, Winrich A. Freiwald  Neuron 
Beautiful Faces Have Variable Reward Value
Attentional Modulations Related to Spatial Gating but Not to Allocation of Limited Resources in Primate V1  Yuzhi Chen, Eyal Seidemann  Neuron  Volume.
Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness in Human Extrastriate Cortex
Fear Conditioning in Humans
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Volume 73, Issue 3, Pages (February 2012)
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Pieter R. Roelfsema, Henk Spekreijse  Neuron 
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 45, Issue 4, Pages (February 2005)
Corticostriatal Output Gating during Selection from Working Memory
Human Orbitofrontal Cortex Represents a Cognitive Map of State Space
Peter Kok, Janneke F.M. Jehee, Floris P. de Lange  Neuron 
Distributed Neural Systems for the Generation of Visual Images
Volume 53, Issue 2, Pages (January 2007)
The Functional Neuroanatomy of Object Agnosia: A Case Study
Brain States: Top-Down Influences in Sensory Processing
Volume 45, Issue 5, Pages (March 2005)
Erie D. Boorman, John P. O’Doherty, Ralph Adolphs, Antonio Rangel 
Volume 47, Issue 5, Pages (September 2005)
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Jan Gläscher, Christian Büchel  Neuron 
High-Resolution fMRI Reveals Laminar Differences in Neurovascular Coupling between Positive and Negative BOLD Responses  Jozien Goense, Hellmut Merkle,
Volume 59, Issue 5, Pages (September 2008)
Normal Movement Selectivity in Autism
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Marlene R. Cohen, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
David Pitcher, Vincent Walsh, Galit Yovel, Bradley Duchaine 
Learning to Simulate Others' Decisions
Michael J. Frank, Brion S. Woroch, Tim Curran  Neuron 
Arielle Tambini, Nicholas Ketz, Lila Davachi  Neuron 
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
John T. Serences, Geoffrey M. Boynton  Neuron 
Phase Locking of Single Neuron Activity to Theta Oscillations during Working Memory in Monkey Extrastriate Visual Cortex  Han Lee, Gregory V. Simpson,
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Mona Spiridon, Nancy Kanwisher  Neuron 
Paying Attention to the Details of Attention
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
A Neural Network Reflecting Decisions about Human Faces
Volume 22, Issue 1, Pages (January 1999)
Volume 50, Issue 1, Pages (April 2006)
An Upside to Reward Sensitivity: The Hippocampus Supports Enhanced Reinforcement Learning in Adolescence  Juliet Y. Davidow, Karin Foerde, Adriana Galván,
Predicting Value of Pain and Analgesia: Nucleus Accumbens Response to Noxious Stimuli Changes in the Presence of Chronic Pain  Marwan N. Baliki, Paul.
Evelina Fedorenko, John Duncan, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Volume 65, Issue 4, Pages (February 2010)
Similarity Breeds Proximity: Pattern Similarity within and across Contexts Is Related to Later Mnemonic Judgments of Temporal Proximity  Youssef Ezzyat,
Patterns of fMRI Activity Dissociate Overlapping Functional Brain Areas that Respond to Biological Motion  Marius V. Peelen, Alison J. Wiggett, Paul E.
Common Prefrontal Regions Coactivate with Dissociable Posterior Regions during Controlled Semantic and Phonological Tasks  Brian T Gold, Randy L Buckner 
Volume 50, Issue 4, Pages (May 2006)
Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Synesthesia
Presentation transcript:

Face Perception Neuron Galit Yovel, Nancy Kanwisher  Neuron  Volume 44, Issue 5, Pages 889-898 (December 2004) DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018

Figure 1 Closely Matched Face and House Tasks Were Used In each task, an image of a face or a house was manipulated in one of two different ways. For the part set, the shapes of the parts (eyes and mouth in faces, windows and doors in houses) were manipulated to generate four different stimuli that differed in parts, but shared the same configuration. For the configuration set, the spacing between these parts was manipulated to generate four stimuli that shared the same parts, but differed in configuration. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)

Figure 2 The Four Hypotheses Tested in the Behavioral Inversion Effect and the fMRI Study The y axis reflects the magnitude of the fMRI response in the FFA or the magnitude of the inversion effect in the behavioral study (performance for upright − inverted stimuli). (A) A higher FFA response and a larger inversion effect for faces than houses on both the configuration and part tasks would support the stimulus (domain)-specificity hypothesis. (B) A higher FFA response and a larger inversion effect for configural than part processing for faces and houses would support the process specificity hypothesis. (C) A higher FFA response and a larger inversion effect to faces than houses and to the configuration task than the part task would support the stimulus and process specificity hypothesis. (D) A higher FFA response and a larger inversion effect for faces than houses and for configuration rather than parts for faces only would support a stimulus and process specificity hypothesis for faces only. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)

Figure 3 Percent Signal Change in the Right and Left FFA for the Upright Face and Upright House Stimuli Showing a Higher Response to Faces than Houses and a Similar Response to the Configuration and Part Task for Both Stimulus Types These findings, in particular in the right FFA, support a domain-specific hypothesis (see Figure 2A). Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between responses to the configural and part tasks. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)

Figure 4 A Similar Right FFA Face Inversion Effect—Drop in fMRI Signal for Inverted Relative to Upright Stimuli—Was Seen on the Part and the Configuration Tasks These findings mirror our behavioral inversion effect findings and suggest that upright-face processing involves the extraction of both configural and part information. Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between responses to the upright and inverted faces. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)

Figure 5 Percent Signal Change in Object-Selective Regions, Showing a Higher Response to the Part Than the Configuration Task This pattern of response differs significantly from the rFFA response that did not differ for parts and configuration (see Figure 3). Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between responses to the configural and part tasks. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)

Figure 6 Lower Performance for Inverted Faces Than for Upright Faces on the Configural and Part Tasks (A) No inversion effect for houses (B) Note that performance levels for the upright conditions were matched across the two tasks for faces and houses. The difference between performance for upright and inverted stimuli (inversion effect) that for the configural and part tasks was present for faces, but not for houses (C), parallels the rFFA findings and suggests that mechanisms of face perception are stimulus specific for faces rather than process specific for configural processing (see Figure 2A). Error bars represent the standard error of the difference between responses to the configural and part tasks. Neuron 2004 44, 889-898DOI: (10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.018)