Animal Behaviour: Feeding the Superorganism

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Nutrition: Rejection Is the Fly’s Protection
Advertisements

Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
Laminopathies: Too Much SUN Is a Bad Thing
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Obesity: Cognitive Impairment and the Failure to ‘Eat Right’
Animal Vision: Rats Watch the Sky
Bacterial Size: Can’t Escape the Long Arm of the Law
Pericycle Current Biology
Social Behaviour: The Personalities of Groups
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Comparative Cognition: Action Imitation Using Episodic Memory
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Cell Biology: Microtubule Collisions to the Rescue
Cell Division: Experiments and Modelling Unite to Resolve the Middle
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Meiosis: Organizing Microtubule Organizers
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Behavioural Genetics: Evolutionary Fingerprint of the ‘Invisible Hand’
Plant Autoimmunity: When Good Things Go Bad
Animal Behavior: The Truman Show for Ants
Phosphoinositide Function in Cytokinesis
Boring algae Current Biology
Growth: A Model for Establishing Cell Size and Shape
Jes Rust, Torsten Wappler  Current Biology 
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Volume 21, Issue 11, Pages R414-R415 (June 2011)
Nutrient sensors Current Biology
The many colours of ‘the dress’
Volume 23, Issue 9, Pages R364-R365 (May 2013)
Neural Odometry: The Discrete Charm of the Entorhinal Cortex
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
Social Evolution: Ant Eggs Lacking Totipotency
Brain Evolution: Getting Better All the Time?
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Colony-level cognition
Gene Evolution: Getting Something from Nothing
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Volume 16, Issue 23, Pages R981-R985 (December 2006)
The Human Genetic History of the Americas: The Final Frontier
Evolution of Eukaryotic DNA Methylation and the Pursuit of Safer Sex
Imaginal discs Current Biology
Fish choose appropriately when and with whom to collaborate
Animal Behavior: Timing in the Wild
Pericycle Current Biology
The social life of corvids
Communal Nutrition in Ants
Patience Current Biology
Membrane Traffic in the Late Steps of Cytokinesis
Meiosis: Checking Chromosomes Pair up Properly
Group Behaviour: Leadership by Those in Need
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages R784-R785 (September 2012)
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Insect Navigation: How Flies Keep Track of Their Snack
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Planar Cell Polarity: A Bridge Too Far?
Horizontal Gene Transfer: Accidental Inheritance Drives Adaptation
Drosophila Connectomics: Mapping the Larval Eye’s Mind
Colony-level cognition
David A. McVea, Keir G. Pearson  Current Biology 
Volume 20, Issue 13, Pages R555-R556 (July 2010)
The social life of corvids
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Endosperm Imprinting: A Child Custody Battle?
A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Animal Behaviour: Feeding the Superorganism Spencer T. Behmer  Current Biology  Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R366-R368 (May 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.033 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Food transfer between different members of a typical ant colony. Foragers, shown in black, give food to reserve ants, shown in blue. Reserve ants pass the food to nurse ants, shown in orange. Nurse ants then pass the food to the larvae, shown in yellow. As food is transferred between workers and nurse ants, sugars and oils can be extracted, but generally little protein is consumed by the workers. Larvae are the main consumers of protein and are also responsible for digesting the protein that eventually is fed to the queen by nurse ants. (Figure courtesy of Brad Vinson.) Current Biology 2009 19, R366-R368DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.033) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 How does the Geometric Framework work? (A) All animals grow best on a particular mixture of protein and carbohydrate. In the Geometric Framework this mixture is called the ‘intake target’ (represented by the red bulls-eye). Foods in the Geometric Framework are represented as trajectories, or ‘rails’, extending outward from the origin. Here two foods are represented. The pink line is a food with a fixed protein:carbohydrate ratio of 3:1, while the gold line is a food with a fixed protein:carbohydrate ratio of 1:4. An animal can reach its intake target by mixing the two foods (feeding is indicated by the small arrows running parallel to the food rails). The grey dotted line represents the protein:carbohydrate ratio that would be obtained if feeding on the two foods was random. Thus, the example shown demonstrates active nutrient regulation. (B) Regulatory rules with respect to each class of nutrient can be established by restricting animals to a range of foods with fixed protein:carbohydrate ratios (the five thin grey lines), measuring the intake point for each diet (the closed circles), and then constructing an ‘intake array’ (the colored lines) by connecting the observed intake points. The red bulls-eye represents the hypothetical intake target from (A), and the three colored lines represent three different regulatory responses. The orange line demonstrates strong carbohydrate regulation, while the blue line shows strong protein regulation. The green line represents a compromise. Here the nutrient in excess, relative to the intake target, is moderately over-eaten, while the nutrient in deficit, relative to the intake target, is moderately under-eaten. Current Biology 2009 19, R366-R368DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.033) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions