Syntax Syntax plays a less prominent role in Baby Talk than does vocabulary. Parents seem to use standard syntax in Baby Talk. Parents’ utterances are.

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
How Children Learn Language. Lec. 3
Advertisements

Understanding Progress in English A Guide for Parents.
Intellectual Development of Toddlers Godbey – Unit 3.
EDU 280 Fall 2014 Language Development: Toddlerhood.
PSY 369: Psycholinguistics Language Acquisition: Learning words, syntax, and more.
Grammar Subject Pronouns
Chapter 4.  Cognition – all mental activities associated with thinking, knowing, remembering, and communicating  Jean Piaget ◦ Theory of Cognitive Development.
Cognitive Development: Language Infants and children face an especially important developmental task with the acquisition of language.
A WALK TO THE JETTY From “Annie John” BY Jamaica Kincaid
Adele E. Goldberg. How argument structure constructions are learned.
Said and told Monday, August 01 st. Said and Told Said have similar meaning Tell.
Chapter 16 Children, the Early Years. Enduring Understandings Unit Six Preschoolers learn through play.
Language Acquisition later stages Morpheme & Syntax Acquisition (Steinberg & Sciarini, pp )
THE IMPORTANCE OF BUILDING SELF-ESTEEM IN YOUNG CHILDREN BY:TIFFANY KERBY.
ESSENTIAL QUESTION: HOW DOES AN INFANT’S BRAIN DEVELOP AND WHAT CAN CAREGIVERS DO TO PROMOTE DEVELOPMENT? Chapter 9: Intellectual Development in Infants.
During the first 6 months of life, a baby’s vocalisations are dominated by crying, cooing, and laughter, which have limited value as a mean of communication.
Sad Movies Sad movies always make me _______. He said he had to work, so I went to the show _______. They turned down the lights and turned the projector.
Vocabulary 7b Thinking Language Intelligence. a methodical, logical rule or procedure that guarantees solving a particular problem. Contrasts with the.
The Giver By Lois Lowry Setting  A society on earth in the far future  Takes place many years in future  It is very strict with many rules and orders.
PARTS OF SPEECH NOTES NOUNS AND VERBS. Nouns Nouns refers to a person, place, thing, or idea. There are several different types of nouns: Common and proper.
Parts of a Sentence. The Sentence is: a two-part thought contains a subject and a predicate an idea John watched. If John watched...
The development of speech production
Saying “I’m Sorry” (and meaning it!)
Second-language learning
Referents and referring expressions Reem Nasser Abdelwahed
BOYS AND GIRLS There are many different kinds of friends. It is nice to have lots of friends Friends are people who talk together and play games. Friends.
BOYS AND GIRLS There are many different kinds of friends. It is nice to have lots of friends Friends are people who talk together and play games. Friends.
Memory and logic in language learning
Module 14 Thought & Language.
Bullying and Feelings © Changing Faces 2009 Registered Charity No Charity registered in Scotland SC
Thought as the basis of speech comprehension
Babies/Young Children
Using Age-Appropriate Comprehensible Input to Engage ELLs in the Home and Classroom Kathy Hart Smith, PhD.
Unit OP 1 Support children with additional needs
Speech and Language Development created by I Abdelnour Information from American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.
Theories of Language Development
Helping Children Learn
Topic 1: Logical behaviourism
Wish Wish is used when the speaker wants reality to be different, to be exactly the opposite. Verb forms similar to those in conditional sentences are.
Supporting Young Children’s Vocabulary
Hey, What’s a Possessive Pronoun? Duh, What’s a Contraction?
Telegraphic speech: two- and three-word utterances
Rationalism versus Empiricism
Understanding Children from 2 to 3 years old.
Wonders High Frequency Words
Preschooler Ch
List 1.
Supporting Young Children’s Vocabulary
Saying “I’m Sorry” (and meaning it!)
Introducing the Ideas One of Six Traits:
Introduction to Word Classes
Intellectual development of infants
Intellectual Development of Toddlers
Class 7.
Read Aloud.
Supporting Young Children’s Vocabulary
Theories of Language Acquisition
TODDLERS 12 MONTHS TO 3 YEARS OLD.
Memory and logic in language learning
Constructivism Constructivism — particularly in its "social" forms — suggests that the learner is much more actively involved in a joint enterprise with.
Agenda- May 11th Finish up the Brochures.
Getting along with others
Supporting Young Children’s Vocabulary
Direct and Indirect Speech
STAGES OF FIRST LANGUAGE ACQUISITION
Thought as the basis of speech comprehension
Quotes from Act 3.
Second-language learning
Presentation on: Referents and referring expressions
Presentation transcript:

Syntax Syntax plays a less prominent role in Baby Talk than does vocabulary. Parents seem to use standard syntax in Baby Talk. Parents’ utterances are strikingly similar to those in the children’s telegraphic stage of speech production, with the focus being on word order.

A mother says ‘Mommy give Tony banana’ instead of the syntactically proper ‘I will give you a banana’. Neither the modal ‘will’ nor the article ‘a’ has been included.

Substituting proper names for personal pronouns is a common feature of Baby Talk that is not usually found in speech between adults. fixed proper nouns are easier for the young child to understand than are items involving shifting speaker–listener relations.

The effect of Parentese and Baby Talk in language learning The studies done on these questions demonstrate a positive but small effect. Other research also provides evidence that Parentese may be effective but only for children who are very young.

Learning abstract words Children begin with the concrete and go on to the abstract. They begin with physical objects (‘mama’, ‘dog’, ‘ball’, ‘table’) and direct activities (‘run’, ‘jump’, ‘play’, ‘give’) then move to relations and statives (‘on’, ‘sitting’).

Then the child learns words involving mental experiences and relations (‘hungry’, ‘hurt’, ‘happy’, ‘want’), which make utterances as: ‘Mary hurt’, ‘John thirsty’, and ‘Kitty want eat’. Later come complex abstract ideas (‘I’, ‘you’, ‘truth’, ‘lie’, ‘honest’, ‘guess’, ‘hope’, ‘idea’, ‘thought’).

How does the child learn abstract words? Simple association is sufficient for an item such as ‘dog’ where the speech sound ‘dog’ is associated with the object ‘dog’. Complex hypothesizing needs to be employed for learning the words for feelings and concepts because such words are not in the physical environment for the child to directly observe.

The child must make inferences from what people say, and on the basis of what happens in the environment and the mind.

How might the words ‘hungry’ and ‘hurt’ be learned? First the child must take note of when such words are spoken by others and the situations in which they occur. The child might cry and the mother might then say, ‘Are you hungry?’ If the child has the feeling of hunger, the child may then guess (after some repeated instances) that ‘hungry’ refers to what he or she is feeling.

The child might point to a banana and the mother might say in response, ‘Do you want a banana?’ and the child might get the meaning of ‘want’. ‘hurt’. The child falls and is bruised on the knee. The father might say, ‘Poor girl. Does it hurt?’ The child’s feeling of pain might then be associated with the word ‘hurt’.

Or, on receiving an injection with a syringe, the child cries, and the father says, ‘It hurts, doesn’t it? Poor child.’

It is up to the child to remember what words were spoken, e. g It is up to the child to remember what words were spoken, e.g. ‘hungry’ and ‘hurt’, and to relate them to particular feelings that the child has experienced in the mind, ‘hunger’ and ‘pain’.

After a number of such instances where certain words (spoken by others) and certain feelings are experienced together, the child will have enough information to make a guess at which sound form relates to which feeling.

Words like ‘lie’ (falsehood) and ‘guess’ must also have particular environmental situations and mental states for the child to bring together. These will be more difficult to identify than feelings because they involve pure (nonfeeling) ideas.

The child will have to make logical inferences from complex situations in order to extract such ideas.

For example, suppose after telling his mother something that he knows is untrue, ‘I didn’t hit her’, the little boy’s mother might say angrily, ‘That’s a lie!’

What the little boy must do to discover the meaning for this sound form of ‘lie’ is to recall the intention and what he knew in his mind before he said what he said. He knows that what he said (‘I didn’t hit her’) did not correspond with what had actually happened before in the world (he did hit her).