Integrating Genomes, Brain and Behavior in the Study of Songbirds

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
F behavioral plasticity F CH8: development of learning in songbirds F behavioral analysis of birdsong F singing in the brain F summary PART 4: BEHAVIORAL.
Advertisements

BIO A simplified phylogeny of major groups of North American passerines (based on Gill 3 rd edition) You are responsible for knowing the following.
Vocalizations Calls vs. Songs Whistled versus Harmonic mobbing.
Central Paper: Nowicki, S. et al. (2002a) Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional.
Summary (Oscines): (2) Birds must practice (sub-, plastic, and ………………………………………… (3) crystallized song) Trimmed, cut, and frozen If not, song performance.
Announcements The search for speciation genes in the house mouse Bettina Harr University of Cologne, Germany (candidate for position in Vertebrate Evolutionary.
Chapter 10 Opener There are both immediate physiological and long-term evolutionary causes for why this male blue-cheeked bee-eater produces vocalizations.
Organization of Song Learning
Teddy A. Wilkin, Ben C. Sheldon  Current Biology 
Convergent Evolution: Gene Sharing by Eukaryotic Plant Pathogens
Cortical Control: Learning from the Lamprey
Transcriptional Memory: Staying in the Loop
Sexual Selection: The Importance of Long-Term Fitness Measures
David M. Schneider, Sarah M.N. Woolley  Neuron 
Sex determination Current Biology
Musical Consonance: The Importance of Harmonicity
Volume 80, Issue 2, Pages (October 2013)
Cell Walls: Monitoring Integrity with THE Kinase
Insights into the Neural and Genetic Basis of Vocal Communication
Learning to Like Your Voice
Madza Farias-Virgens, Stephanie A. White  Neuron 
Lucas Y. Tian, Michael S. Brainard  Neuron 
Rhea R. Kimpo, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
Integrating Genomes, Brain and Behavior in the Study of Songbirds
Deconstructing Memory in Drosophila
Volume 27, Issue 18, Pages R1020-R1023 (September 2017)
Sleep: How Many Switches Does It Take To Turn Off the Lights?
András Liker, Robert P. Freckleton, Tamás Székely  Current Biology 
Sex determination Current Biology
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Plant Physiology: The Venus Flytrap Counts on Secretion
Auditory Neuroscience: How to Encode Microsecond Differences
Volume 90, Issue 4, Pages (May 2016)
Volume 28, Issue 5, Pages e6 (March 2018)
Silent Reading: Does the Brain ‘Hear’ Both Speech and Voices?
Distributed Time-Domain Representations in the Birdsong System
Huimeng Lei, Richard Mooney  Neuron 
David M. Schneider, Sarah M.N. Woolley  Neuron 
Sex ratios and social evolution
Volume 90, Issue 4, Pages (May 2016)
Diminished FoxP2 Levels Affect Dopaminergic Modulation of Corticostriatal Signaling Important to Song Variability  Malavika Murugan, Stephen Harward,
Neurogenetics: Short-Circuiting Sexually Dimorphic Behaviors
Gustatory Receptors: Not Just for Good Taste
Neuronal Decision-Making Circuits
Charles Choi, Michael N. Nitabach  Current Biology 
Vocal Development: How Marmoset Infants Express Their Feelings
Do birds have the capacity for fun?
Sex Recognition through Midflight Mating Duets in Culex Mosquitoes Is Mediated by Acoustic Distortion  Ben Warren, Gabriella Gibson, Ian J. Russell  Current.
Sexual Selection and Condition-Dependent Mate Preferences
Climate Change: A Hybrid Zone Moves North
Volume 25, Issue 23, Pages R1116-R1121 (December 2015)
The social life of corvids
Le-Qing Wu, J. David Dickman  Current Biology 
Centrosome Size: Scaling Without Measuring
Volume 26, Issue 17, Pages (September 2016)
Sensorimotor Neuroscience: Motor Precision Meets Vision
Raghav Rajan, Allison J. Doupe  Current Biology 
Context-Dependent Functions of Avian Duets Revealed by Microphone-Array Recordings and Multispeaker Playback  Daniel J. Mennill, Sandra L. Vehrencamp 
Volume 28, Issue 5, Pages R204-R205 (March 2018)
Volume 46, Issue 1, Pages 4-7 (April 2005)
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Locomotion Control: Brainstem Circuits Satisfy the Need for Speed
Volume 23, Issue 1, Pages R12-R15 (January 2013)
Remembering the Prolonged Cold of Winter
Temporal coordination signals coalition quality
Rhea R. Kimpo, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
Evolution: A Paradigm Shift in Snake Sex Chromosome Genetics
The social life of corvids
Sarah C. Woolley, Raghav Rajan, Mati Joshua, Allison J. Doupe  Neuron 
Presentation transcript:

Integrating Genomes, Brain and Behavior in the Study of Songbirds David F. Clayton, Christopher N. Balakrishnan, Sarah E. London  Current Biology  Volume 19, Issue 18, Pages R865-R873 (September 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Zebra finches. A zebra finch male (center) flanked by two females. Zebra finches are highly social, maintaining lifelong monogamous pair bonds in large colonies. Current Biology 2009 19, R865-R873DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Overview of songbird phylogeny. Note the position of family Estrildidae (which includes the zebra finch) as one of the ‘vocal learner’ families, in contrast to the ‘non-learner’ suboscines. Some families and species from each superfamily are listed as representatives (in total there are 122 families within the five major superfamilies of the Passerida). Drawings of a male of one species contained within the representative families are included (drawings not to scale). Estrildidae: zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata); Fringillidae: purple finch (Carpodacus purpureus); Icteridae: Brewer's blackbird (Euphagus cyanocephalus); Sittidae: red-breasted nuthatch (Sitta canadensis); Troglodytidae: winter wren (Troglodytes troglodytes); Muscicapidae: vivid niltava (Niltava vivida); Sturnidae: European starling (Sturnus vulgaris); Paridae: yellow-cheeked tit (Parus spilonotus); Phylloscopidae: Tickell's leaf warbler (Phylloscopus affinis); Corvidae: western scrub jay (Aphelocoma californica); Paradisaeidae: lesser bird-of-paradise (Paradisaea minor); Pipridae: golden-collared manakin (Manacus vitellinus); Tyrannidae: brown-backed chat-tyrant (Ochthoeca fumicolor). Current Biology 2009 19, R865-R873DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Core elements of the ‘song circuit’. Several brain centers responsible for the production of learned vocal signals in songbirds are shown. Red lines indicate the motor output pathway, descending from the nucleus known as HVC (used as a proper name) to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA). Blue lines indicate an anterior forebrain loop needed for song learning, which begins with projections from a distinct set of neurons in HVC to a specialized region within the basal ganglia known as Area X. Area X projects to the medial dorsolateral nucleus of the anterior thalamus (DLM), which projects back up to the lateral portion of the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (LMAN). LMAN projects back onto neurons in RA that also receive input from HVC; this projection also sends collaterals to Area X. Current Biology 2009 19, R865-R873DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Spectrograms of songs from several songbird species. (A) Songs of three unrelated male zebra finches. Each zebra finch sings a unique song, observable by the distinct structure of each song element, or syllable, and by the ordering of those syllables. Note the typical ‘harmonic stack’ structure to the songs (visible as alternating black and white horizontal bars), individual variation in syllable structure, number of syllables, and total duration of song. (B) Canary song featuring pure-tone like syllables, syllable repetition, a multitude of different syllables, and a long song duration. (C) Tonal whistles of the northern cardinal, a species in which both sexes sing. (D) Song sparrow song highlighting syllable diversity; tones and harmonic stacks are visible. Elements can be repeated, and there are several types of syllables that comprise the song. (D) Songs of brood parasitic Cameroon indigobirds. There are two types of song: ‘non-mimicry’ songs, which are indigobird-specific, and ‘mimicry’ songs, which have syllables that are copied from the host species song. Note that the conspecific non-mimicry song is more complex than the mimicry song; it is composed of several distinct syllables types, many with rapid frequency modulation, and is longer in duration than the mimicry song. Current Biology 2009 19, R865-R873DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 5 Genome–brain–behavior relationships. The genome influences behavior of individuals indirectly by encoding proteins and RNAs, which are components of cells and circuits, which make up the neural systems responsible for behavioral control. Individual behavior is subject to natural selection, leading over generations to the establishment of gene sequences associated with successful behavioral phenotypes. Environment affects behavior in physiological time via the responses of neural systems, and it affects the genome in evolutionary time as a component of natural selection. Physiological processes can lead to epigenetic modifications of the genome. An individual's environment includes the behavior of other interacting individuals. Social organization emerges out of the behavioral interactions of individuals. Current Biology 2009 19, R865-R873DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions