Time perception: Brain time or event time?

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Perception: Shaping What We See
Advertisements

Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Molecular evolution: Actin's long lost relative found
Cytoskeletal Chirality: Swirling Cells Tell Left from Right
Circadian Rhythms: To Sync or Not To Sync
Circadian Rhythms: To Sync or Not To Sync
Time perception: Brain time or event time?
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages R1022-R1023 (December 2009)
The visual perception of motion
Oogenesis: Matrix Revolutions
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Microbiology: Mixing Wine, Chocolate, and Coffee
Synaptic plasticity: Going through phases with LTP
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Mosaic and regulative development: two faces of one coin
Drosophila development: Scalloped and Vestigial take wing
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Molecular motors: Kinesin's string variable
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
Transcription: Identification of a prime suspect
Independent Aftereffects of Attention and Motion
Calcium Signalling: Calcium Goes Global
Robert J. Snowden, Alan B. Milne  Current Biology 
Motor Networks: Shifting Coalitions
Adaptive Surround Modulation in Cortical Area MT
The many colours of ‘the dress’
Cellular signalling: Stressing the importance of PIP3
Publication metrics and success on the academic job market
Motion Processing: How Low Can You Go?
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Sense of agency Current Biology
Time Perception: Space–Time in the Brain
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Integration of Local Features into Global Shapes
Non-cortical magnitude coding of space and time by pigeons
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Mosaic and regulative development: two faces of one coin
Shin'ya Nishida, Alan Johnston  Current Biology 
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages R188-R189 (March 2015)
Stephen G. Lomber, Blake E. Butler  Current Biology 
Meiosis: Checking Chromosomes Pair up Properly
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Volume 14, Issue 3, Pages (February 2004)
Asynchronous processing in vision
Visual aftereffects Current Biology
Volume 25, Issue 10, Pages R394-R397 (May 2015)
Myosin structure: Does the tail wag the dog?
The Cellular Organization of Zebrafish Visuomotor Circuits
Visual Adaptation of the Perception of Causality
Interaction between different types of visual information
Systems neuroscience: The slowly sleeping slab and slice
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Volume 7, Issue 6, Pages R147-R151 (June 2000)
Vision research: Losing sight of eye dominance
Multisensory Integration: Maintaining the Perception of Synchrony
Calcium signalling Current Biology
Visual selection: Neurons that make up their minds
Filling-in afterimage colors between the lines
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
The functional architecture of attention
Planar Cell Polarity: A Bridge Too Far?
Perceptual Learning: Is V1 up to the Task?
Visual Perception: One World from Two Eyes
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Transcriptional control: Imprinting insulation
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Animal navigation: Birds as geometers?
Motion processing: Where is the naso-temporal asymmetry?
A way of selectively degrading colour constancy demonstrates the experience dependence of colour vision  Eli Brenner, Frans W. Cornelissen  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Time perception: Brain time or event time? Alan Johnston, Shin'ya Nishida  Current Biology  Volume 11, Issue 11, Pages R427-R430 (June 2001) DOI: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00252-4

Fig. 1 Green dots moving upwards and red dots moving downwards do not seem to change in synchrony. The changes in direction have to precede the changes in colour by around 80 milliseconds to achieve temporal binding. The arrow indicates the direction of time past. Current Biology 2001 11, R427-R430DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00252-4)

Fig. 2 The contingent aftereffect: after an observer has adapted to alternating clockwise rotating red patterns and anticlockwise rotating green patterns, then static red patterns appear to rotate anticlockwise and static green pattern rotates clockwise. Arnold et al.[7] manipulated the relative phases of the changes in motion direction and colour during adaptation in separate trials (shown on the left), and then measured the direction and magnitude of the contingent aftereffect. They found that the pattern of adaptation, as a function of the relative phase of the adapting colour and motion signal, is biased overall, suggesting that colour processing precedes motion processing by around 80 milliseconds. Current Biology 2001 11, R427-R430DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00252-4)

Fig. 3 A motion delay could give rise to different strength of association between colour and motion, but another possibility is that there is a temporal asymmetry in the strength of the linkage. Either situation could generate contingent aftereffects for 90° or 270° physical phase shifts. Current Biology 2001 11, R427-R430DOI: (10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00252-4)