Jingping P. Xu, Zijiang J. He, Teng Leng Ooi  Current Biology 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 54, Issue 6, Pages (June 2007)
Advertisements

Blur and Disparity Are Complementary Cues to Depth
Volume 32, Issue 2, Pages (October 2001)
Long-Term Speeding in Perceptual Switches Mediated by Attention-Dependent Plasticity in Cortical Visual Processing  Satoru Suzuki, Marcia Grabowecky 
Development of Direction Selectivity in Mouse Cortical Neurons
Decision Making during the Psychological Refractory Period
Natalia Zaretskaya, Andreas Bartels  Current Biology 
A push-pull treatment for strengthening the ‘lazy eye’ in amblyopia
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Perceptual Echoes at 10 Hz in the Human Brain
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Masayuki Haruta, Yoshio Hata  Current Biology 
Ryota Kanai, Naotsugu Tsuchiya, Frans A.J. Verstraten  Current Biology 
Minami Ito, Gerald Westheimer, Charles D Gilbert  Neuron 
Michael L. Morgan, Gregory C. DeAngelis, Dora E. Angelaki  Neuron 
Benjamin Thompson, Behzad Mansouri, Lisa Koski, Robert F. Hess 
Volume 26, Issue 3, Pages (February 2016)
Saccadic suppression precedes visual motion analysis
Young Children Do Not Integrate Visual and Haptic Form Information
Blur and Disparity Are Complementary Cues to Depth
Disruption of Perceptual Learning by a Brief Practice Break
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Learning to Link Visual Contours
Adaptation without Plasticity
Serial Dependence in the Perception of Faces
Dichoptic training enables the adult amblyopic brain to learn
Volume 18, Issue 24, Pages (December 2008)
Dynamics of a Memory Trace: Effects of Sleep on Consolidation
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Development of Direction Selectivity in Mouse Cortical Neurons
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Visual Sensitivity Can Scale with Illusory Size Changes
Peng Zhang, Min Bao, Miyoung Kwon, Sheng He, Stephen A. Engel 
The Normalization Model of Attention
Adaptation without Plasticity
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Martijn Barendregt, Ben M. Harvey, Bas Rokers, Serge O. Dumoulin 
Attention Reorients Periodically
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Category Selectivity in the Ventral Visual Pathway Confers Robustness to Clutter and Diverted Attention  Leila Reddy, Nancy Kanwisher  Current Biology 
Keith A. May, Li Zhaoping, Paul B. Hibbard  Current Biology 
Binocular Vision: The Eyes Add and Subtract
Volume 17, Issue 15, Pages (August 2007)
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
Sung Jun Joo, Geoffrey M. Boynton, Scott O. Murray  Current Biology 
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Cross-Modal Associative Mnemonic Signals in Crow Endbrain Neurons
Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Søren K. Andersen, Steven A. Hillyard, Matthias M. Müller 
Manuel Jan Roth, Matthis Synofzik, Axel Lindner  Current Biology 
Taosheng Liu, Franco Pestilli, Marisa Carrasco  Neuron 
A Visual Sense of Number
Volume 22, Issue 5, Pages (March 2012)
Yuko Yotsumoto, Takeo Watanabe, Yuka Sasaki  Neuron 
Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern
Motion Adaptation and the Velocity Coding of Natural Scenes
Volume 25, Issue 6, Pages (March 2015)
Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Motion-Induced Blindness and Motion Streak Suppression
Presentation transcript:

Effectively Reducing Sensory Eye Dominance with a Push-Pull Perceptual Learning Protocol  Jingping P. Xu, Zijiang J. He, Teng Leng Ooi  Current Biology  Volume 20, Issue 20, Pages 1864-1868 (October 2010) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043 Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Current Biology 2010 20, 1864-1868DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 A Conceptual Cortical Model of Binocular Interaction (A) At the first level, monocular units with different orientation preference from each eye inhibit one another. At the second level, monocular units from each eye with the same orientation preference converge. Interocular inhibition is activated when the two eyes are stimulated with orthogonal gratings. (B) Sensory eye dominance with strong inhibition of the left eye (LE). When the two eyes are presented with orthogonal gratings of equal contrast, the right eye's (RE) grating (strong eye) is perceived while the LE's grating (weak eye) is suppressed because of the stronger inhibition on the LE's monocular units. Current Biology 2010 20, 1864-1868DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Experimental Protocols for Sensory Eye Dominance and Perceptual Learning (A and B) Orthogonal gratings used to measure the balance contrast in the RE and LE, whose difference defines the sensory eye dominance. (C) Push-pull protocol. The white rectangular frame acts as a cue to attract transient attention, causing the (vertical) grating in the weak eye to be perceived while the (horizontal) grating in the strong eye is suppressed. (D) Push-only protocol. The stimulus presentation sequence is the same as in the push-pull protocol, except no grating is presented to the strong eye. Current Biology 2010 20, 1864-1868DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Outcomes of the Perceptual Learning Protocols on Sensory Eye Dominance of Seven Observers Trained with the Interleaved Procedure (A) The average interocular balance contrast with the push-pull protocol. The interocular balance contrast was obtained with grating whose orientation was the same as, or orthogonal to, the grating used in the training, respectively, and was measured before and after each day's training. The balance contrast reduces with days in training when tested with the same orientation grating. (B) The interocular average balance contrast with the push-only protocol. The interocular balance contrast does not change with training. (C) Sensory eye dominance (SED; measured before each day's training session) reduces with the push-pull but not the push-only protocol. Both (A) and (B) also include the average data of observers (n = 3) trained in a sequential procedure (plus and cross symbols; error bars are not shown to reduce clutter). See also Figures S1A and S1B. Error bars indicate 1 standard error of the mean (SEM). Current Biology 2010 20, 1864-1868DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Outcomes of the Perceptual Learning Protocols on Unrelated Monocular and Binocular Functions (A) Reduction in monocular contrast threshold at the push-pull (black bars) and push-only (gray bars) training locations in the weak and strong eye. (B) Reduction in monocular orientation discrimination threshold. (C and D) The reduction in stereo threshold and reaction time, respectively, at the push-pull (black bar), push-only (gray bar), and an untrained (open bar) location. ∗p < 0.05. Error bars indicate 1 SEM. Current Biology 2010 20, 1864-1868DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.043) Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions