Communal Nutrition in Ants

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Advertisements

Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Auguste M.P. von Bayern, Nathan J. Emery  Current Biology 
Volume 23, Issue 19, Pages (October 2013)
Noise Pollution Changes Avian Communities and Species Interactions
Volume 27, Issue 15, Pages e4 (August 2017)
Volume 16, Issue 22, Pages (November 2006)
Lycaenid Caterpillar Secretions Manipulate Attendant Ant Behavior
How Ants Use Vision When Homing Backward
Michael Boots, Dylan Childs, Daniel C. Reuman, Michael Mealor 
Eye–Hand Coordination: Learning a New Trick
Vision: Attention Makes the Cup Flow Over
Somatosensory Precision in Speech Production
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Morphogens: Precise Outputs from a Variable Gradient
Representation of Object Weight in Human Ventral Visual Cortex
Ant Navigation: One-Way Routes Rather Than Maps
Volume 16, Issue 3, Pages (February 2006)
Putting the Balance Back in Diet
Colin J. Palmer, Colin W.G. Clifford  Current Biology 
The Jellyfish Cassiopea Exhibits a Sleep-like State
Volume 24, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Volume 24, Issue 4, Pages (February 2014)
Volume 19, Issue 6, Pages R233-R234 (March 2009)
Nutrient sensors Current Biology
Evolutionary Conditions for the Emergence of Communication in Robots
Volume 19, Issue 22, Pages (December 2009)
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Reinforcement Can Overcome Gene Flow during Speciation in Drosophila
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Target Detection Is Enhanced by Polarization Vision in a Fiddler Crab
Colony-level cognition
Marr's vision: Twenty-five years on
Gene Regulation: Stable Noise
BOLD fMRI Correlation Reflects Frequency-Specific Neuronal Correlation
Cornelia Buehlmann, Paul Graham, Bill S. Hansson, Markus Knaden 
Can We Sustainably Harvest Ivory?
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages R1106-R1107 (October 2017)
Sonal S. Joshi, Victoria H. Meller  Current Biology 
Left Habenular Activity Attenuates Fear Responses in Larval Zebrafish
Investigating Physical Cognition in Rooks, Corvus frugilegus
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
Animal Behaviour: Feeding the Superorganism
Volume 23, Issue 21, Pages R963-R965 (November 2013)
Noise Pollution Changes Avian Communities and Species Interactions
Auguste M.P. von Bayern, Nathan J. Emery  Current Biology 
Cetaceans on a Molecular Fast Track to Ultrasonic Hearing
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Volume 26, Issue 11, Pages (June 2016)
Early Life: Embracing the RNA World
Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon 
Context-Dependent Functions of Avian Duets Revealed by Microphone-Array Recordings and Multispeaker Playback  Daniel J. Mennill, Sandra L. Vehrencamp 
Public Versus Personal Information for Mate Copying in an Invertebrate
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
A Maternal Effect on Queen Production in Honeybees
Rapid Evolution of the Cerebellum in Humans and Other Great Apes
Collective Motion and Cannibalism in Locust Migratory Bands
Geographic Variation of Caste Structure among Ant Populations
Texture density adaptation and visual number revisited
Basal bodies Current Biology
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Social Immunity Current Biology
A Role for S6 Kinase and Serotonin in Postmating Dietary Switch and Balance of Nutrients in D. melanogaster  Misha A. Vargas, Ningguang Luo, Atsushi Yamaguchi,
Visual Crowding Is Correlated with Awareness
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Martin Müller, Rüdiger Wehner  Current Biology 
Presentation transcript:

Communal Nutrition in Ants Audrey Dussutour, Stephen J. Simpson  Current Biology  Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages 740-744 (May 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Protein and Carbohydrate Collection Measured during Choice Experiments (A and B) Empty circles and crosses represent the amount of protein (P) and carbohydrate (C) collected for each replicate for colonies without larvae and with larvae present from the outset of the experiment, respectively. Full circles and full squares show the mean values. The “random” outcomes indicate the expected intake trajectories if feeding had occurred indiscriminately between the two foods in a food pairing treatment (dotted black lines). The solid black lines represent the observed intake trajectory. The solid gray lines correspond to the two foods in a food pairing treatment. Note how colonies with larvae converged upon the same point of nutrient collection in all treatments. Colonies without larvae regulated nutrient collection to a lower P:C ratio than did colonies with larvae, reflecting the increased protein needs of those colonies, and were unable to compensate for the most extreme dilution, indicating a role of larvae in colony nutrient regulation. Three-way ANOVAs: choice effect on food collected and P:C ratio, F1,108 = 0.38 (p = 0.534) and F1,108 = 0.09 (p = 0.781); dilution effect on P:C ratio, F2,108 = 0.01 (p = 0.994); larval effect on P:C ratio and food collected, F1,108 = 792.79 (p < 0.001) and F1,108 = 939.12 (p < 0.001); dilution × larvae effect on food collected, F2,108 = 178.79 (p < 0.001). Current Biology 2009 19, 740-744DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Cumulative Protein and Carbohydrates Collected by Ants Colonies were provided with one of five diets that differed in their ratio of protein to carbohydrate at 2 day intervals over 50 days (the results were adjusted to ant mortality). Each diet is represented as a dotted line in the protein/carbohydrates plane in (A) and (B). Within each time interval, the nutrient collection points are connected with lines to and from collection arrays, which demonstrate the nutrient balancing strategy. The inserted images in (A) and (B) show examples of the amount of food left after 2 days. The amount of diet collected by ants at 2 day intervals over 50 days is shown in (C) and (D). Four-way ANOVAs: time effect on the amount of food collected, F24,960 = 106.82 (p < 0.001); time × larvae effect, F24,960 = 8.49 (p < 0.001); time × diet effect, F96,960 = 6.66 (p < 0.001); time × larvae × diet effect, F96,960 = 3.14 (p < 0.001); larvae effect, F1,40 = 63.00 (p < 0.001); diet effect, F4,40 = 17.48 (p < 0.001); larvae × diet effect, F4,40 = 0.60 (p = 0.661). Current Biology 2009 19, 740-744DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Protein and Carbohydrates Collected and Ingested, i.e., Not Taken out and Placed on a Waste Dump by Ants after 50 Days (A and B) Ants were provided with one of five diets differing in their ratio of protein to carbohydrate over 50 days. Triangles show the amount of C and P rejected from the food collected. Note how colonies with larvae from the outset were more effective than colonies without larvae in manipulating the composition of collected food by retaining the limiting nutrient and rejecting the excess nutrient in collected food (shaded triangles). The intake target collected is the amount of carbohydrates and protein collected during the choice experiment. The intake target ingested is the intake target once the correction for carbohydrate extraction from the foods collected in the choice experiments is made. Results are presented as intake per ant to adjust for mortality. Three-way ANOVA: diet effect on the amount of stock, F4,40 = 78.61 (p < 0.001); larvae effect, F1,40 = 96.20 (p < 0.001). Four-way ANOVAs with repeated measures: manipulation effect on C and P quantity in the stock, F1,40 = 4068.94 (p < 0.001) and F1,40 = 755.13 (p < 0.001); manipulation × larvae effect × diet effect on C quantity, F1,40 = 8.45 (p < 0.001). Current Biology 2009 19, 740-744DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Colony Performance Ants were provided with one of five diets differing in their ratio of protein to carbohydrate over 50 days. (A and B) Mortality for colonies with and without larvae from the beginning of the experiment. (C) Number of larvae produced after 50 days. (D) Number of larvae that hatched after 50 days in colonies in which 60 larvae were introduced at the beginning of the experiment. Three-way ANOVAs: diet effect on the number of dead ants, F4,40 = 25.99 (p < 0.001); larvae × diet effect, F4,40 = 1.28 (p = 0.292); larvae effect, F1,40 = 7.56 (p = 0.009); diet effect on the number of larvae, F4,40 = 47.42 (p < 0.001); larvae × diet effect, F4,40 = 2.29 (p = 0.076); larvae effect, F1,40 = 11.56 (p = 0.002). One-way ANOVA: diet effect on number of ants that hatched, F4,29 = 82.81 (p < 0.001). Current Biology 2009 19, 740-744DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.015) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions