Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Stimulus Visibility Reflected in Microsaccade Activity Jie Cui 1, Melanie Wilke 2, Nikos Logothetis 3, David Leopold 2, Hualou Liang 1 1 School of Health.
Advertisements

Voice Cells in the Primate Temporal Lobe
Rapid and Persistent Adaptability of Human Oculomotor Control in Response to Simulated Central Vision Loss  MiYoung Kwon, Anirvan S. Nandy, Bosco S. Tjan 
Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation
Ziad M. Hafed, Chih-Yang Chen  Current Biology 
A Sensorimotor Role for Traveling Waves in Primate Visual Cortex
A Multisensory Network for Olfactory Processing
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
One-Dimensional Dynamics of Attention and Decision Making in LIP
Neuronal Correlates of Metacognition in Primate Frontal Cortex
A Source for Feature-Based Attention in the Prefrontal Cortex
Signal, Noise, and Variation in Neural and Sensory-Motor Latency
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Volume 58, Issue 3, Pages (May 2008)
Decoding Wakefulness Levels from Typical fMRI Resting-State Data Reveals Reliable Drifts between Wakefulness and Sleep  Enzo Tagliazucchi, Helmut Laufs 
Linking Electrical Stimulation of Human Primary Visual Cortex, Size of Affected Cortical Area, Neuronal Responses, and Subjective Experience  Jonathan.
Volume 62, Issue 5, Pages (June 2009)
Choice Certainty Is Informed by Both Evidence and Decision Time
Heather L. Dean, Maureen A. Hagan, Bijan Pesaran  Neuron 
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 87, Issue 1, Pages (July 2015)
The Primate Cerebellum Selectively Encodes Unexpected Self-Motion
Alteration of Visual Perception prior to Microsaccades
Volume 81, Issue 6, Pages (March 2014)
Neural Correlates of Knowledge: Stable Representation of Stimulus Associations across Variations in Behavioral Performance  Adam Messinger, Larry R. Squire,
Volume 74, Issue 5, Pages (June 2012)
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Roman F. Loonis, Scott L. Brincat, Evan G. Antzoulatos, Earl K. Miller 
Feature- and Order-Based Timing Representations in the Frontal Cortex
Volume 18, Issue 4, Pages (January 2017)
A Role for the Superior Colliculus in Decision Criteria
Volume 66, Issue 4, Pages (May 2010)
Nicolas Catz, Peter W. Dicke, Peter Thier  Current Biology 
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Huihui Zhou, Robert Desimone  Neuron 
Volume 54, Issue 6, Pages (June 2007)
Sensory Population Decoding for Visually Guided Movements
Rethinking Motor Learning and Savings in Adaptation Paradigms: Model-Free Memory for Successful Actions Combines with Internal Models  Vincent S. Huang,
Volume 28, Issue 15, Pages e5 (August 2018)
Independent Category and Spatial Encoding in Parietal Cortex
Katherine M. Armstrong, Jamie K. Fitzgerald, Tirin Moore  Neuron 
Feature-based attention in visual cortex
Caleb E. Strait, Tommy C. Blanchard, Benjamin Y. Hayden  Neuron 
Volume 91, Issue 5, Pages (September 2016)
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Spatiotopic Visual Maps Revealed by Saccadic Adaptation in Humans
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Volume 77, Issue 6, Pages (March 2013)
Xiaomo Chen, Marc Zirnsak, Tirin Moore  Cell Reports 
Microsaccades Counteract Visual Fading during Fixation
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Timescales of Inference in Visual Adaptation
Stephen V. David, Benjamin Y. Hayden, James A. Mazer, Jack L. Gallant 
Volume 76, Issue 4, Pages (November 2012)
The Normalization Model of Attention
Volume 72, Issue 6, Pages (December 2011)
Cognitive neural prosthetics
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Volume 24, Issue 8, Pages e6 (August 2018)
Transient Slow Gamma Synchrony Underlies Hippocampal Memory Replay
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Volume 78, Issue 4, Pages (May 2013)
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Volume 99, Issue 1, Pages e4 (July 2018)
Supratim Ray, John H.R. Maunsell  Neuron 
Neuroscience: Tiny Eye Movements Link Vision and Attention
Richard A. Andersen, He Cui  Neuron 
Matthew R. Roesch, Adam R. Taylor, Geoffrey Schoenbaum  Neuron 
Volume 28, Issue 18, Pages e3 (September 2018)
Presentation transcript:

Microsaccades: a neurophysiological analysis Susana Martinez-Conde, Stephen L. Macknik, Xoana G. Troncoso, David H. Hubel  Trends in Neurosciences  Volume 32, Issue 9, Pages 463-475 (September 2009) DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006 Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Microsaccade generation mechanisms. (a) Microsaccades follow the saccadic main sequence, suggesting a common generator for microsaccades and saccades. Saccades and microsaccades recorded during free-viewing (blue) follow the same main sequence as those produced during fixation (red). (Note that some blue dots are obscured by the superimposed red dots; n=159,874 saccades and microsaccades combined for 8 subjects. Taken from Ref. [14].) (b) Peak discharge of an SC neuron during microsaccades as a function of their horizontal and vertical amplitudes. The neuron preferred microsaccades directed to the lower right quadrant. Modified from Ref. [25]. (c) Microsaccade probability during 1s of fixation before (gray) and after (black) SC inactivation. Inactivation reduced microsaccade production. Modified from Ref. [25]. Trends in Neurosciences 2009 32, 463-475DOI: (10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Microsaccades counteract visual fading and filling-in. Microsaccade rates before transitions towards visibility (unfilled scotomas, red) versus fading (filled-in scotomas, blue). The horizontal line indicates the average rate of microsaccades during the session. Shaded areas indicate SEM between subjects (n=6). The gray dots at the top indicate the bins where the unfilled averages are significantly different from the filled-in averages (one-sample one-tailed paired t-test, ≤0.05). From Ref. [7]. The model of Hafed et al. for microsaccade generation might explain the correlations between increases or decreases in microsaccade rates and visible or fading percepts [25]. Trends in Neurosciences 2009 32, 463-475DOI: (10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure I Distribution of microsaccade amplitudes in humans and primates. The data plots from two recent studies are superimposed [18,25]. Microsaccade amplitudes are comparable for humans and primates, irrespective of the eye-tracking method used (video tracking vs. magnetic search coil). Many of the microsaccades detected are considerably larger than 12arcmin. We should also note that prolonged visual fixation typically results in larger microsaccades than those depicted in this figure (data not shown; see Figure 3B of Ref. [14] or Figure 1E of Ref. [7]). Modified from Refs [18,25]. Trends in Neurosciences 2009 32, 463-475DOI: (10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure II Neural responses to flashes versus microsaccades in Martinez-Conde et al. 2002 [44] (left panel) and Kagan et al. 2008 [45] (right panel). The accompanying tables confirm that the results from both studies are consistent with each other, regardless of the specific metrics used to compare them. The calculations were directly estimated from the population graphs in both papers (the results published by Martinez-Conde et al. [44] were first calculated numerically for each neuron, and then averaged across neurons). Trends in Neurosciences 2009 32, 463-475DOI: (10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.006) Copyright © 2009 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions