Evolutionary Psychology of Spatial Representations in the Hominidae

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Advertisements

Animal Cognition: Monkeys Pass the Mirror Test
Auguste M.P. von Bayern, Nathan J. Emery  Current Biology 
Mark J. Costello, Chhaya Chaudhary
How the Opinion of Others Affects Our Valuation of Objects
Victoria F. Ratcliffe, David Reby  Current Biology 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Elise A. Piazza, Marius Cătălin Iordan, Casey Lew-Williams 
Choosing Goals, Not Rules: Deciding among Rule-Based Action Plans
Episodic Memory: Rats Master Multiple Memories
Volume 27, Issue 22, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Autism: Face-Processing Clues to Inheritance
Chimpanzees Trust Their Friends
Life History: The Energy-Efficient Orangutan
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Janne-Tuomas Seppänen, Jukka T. Forsman  Current Biology 
Behavioural Genetics: Evolutionary Fingerprint of the ‘Invisible Hand’
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages R827-R828 (September 2013)
Volume 26, Issue 13, Pages (July 2016)
Volume 19, Issue 18, Pages (September 2009)
Dogs Can Discriminate Emotional Expressions of Human Faces
Road crossing in chimpanzees: A risky business
Children, but Not Chimpanzees, Prefer to Collaborate
Tamar Gutnick, Ruth A. Byrne, Binyamin Hochner, Michael Kuba 
Visual motion interferes with lexical decision on motion words
Avi J.H. Chanales, Ashima Oza, Serra E. Favila, Brice A. Kuhl 
Selection of Effective Stone Tools by Wild Bearded Capuchin Monkeys
Volume 22, Issue 18, Pages (September 2012)
Bonobos Prefer Individuals that Hinder Others over Those that Help
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Integration Trumps Selection in Object Recognition
Working memory without consciousness
Volume 19, Issue 6, Pages (March 2009)
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Restorative Justice in Children
The Social Dominance Paradox
Volume 19, Issue 3, Pages (February 2009)
Daniel Hanus, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Jannath Begum Ali, Charles Spence, Andrew J. Bremner  Current Biology 
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
The Evolutionary Origins of Human Patience: Temporal Preferences in Chimpanzees, Bonobos, and Human Adults  Alexandra G. Rosati, Jeffrey R. Stevens, Brian.
Rooks Use Stones to Raise the Water Level to Reach a Floating Worm
Reconstructing the Evolution of Laughter in Great Apes and Humans
Auguste M.P. von Bayern, Nathan J. Emery  Current Biology 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Imitation recognition in great apes
Dongjun He, Daniel Kersten, Fang Fang  Current Biology 
Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory  Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow,
A Causal Link between Visual Spatial Attention and Reading Acquisition
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Federica Amici, Filippo Aureli, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Akinori Mitani, Mingyuan Dong, Takaki Komiyama  Current Biology 
Self-Control in Chimpanzees Relates to General Intelligence
Hermaphroditic Sex Allocation Evolves When Mating Opportunities Change
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Rapid Evolution of the Cerebellum in Humans and Other Great Apes
Hippocampal-Prefrontal Theta Oscillations Support Memory Integration
Kazumichi Matsumiya, Satoshi Shioiri  Current Biology 
Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs
Jonathan Redshaw, Thomas Suddendorf  Current Biology 
Li Zhaoping, Nathalie Guyader  Current Biology 
Will Travel for Food: Spatial Discounting in Two New World Monkeys
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
Spatiotemporal Neural Pattern Similarity Supports Episodic Memory
Presentation transcript:

Evolutionary Psychology of Spatial Representations in the Hominidae Daniel B.M. Haun, Josep Call, Gabriele Janzen, Stephen C. Levinson  Current Biology  Volume 16, Issue 17, Pages 1736-1740 (September 2006) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049 Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Experimental Conditions (Top) Adult orangutan male performing the task. (Bottom) Experimental conditions. The X indicates the hidden reward before and after occlusion. Place: During occlusion, the experimenter (E) switches two containers while the reward stays in its original location. Feature: E again switches two containers during occlusion but moves the reward with its container. Current Biology 2006 16, 1736-1740DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Strategy Preferences: Performance Mean percent correct (± SE) for the place and feature conditions only. Means are adjusted for the respective chance levels of the different subject groups: 33.33% for 3-year-old human children and nonhuman great apes (1 out of 3 containers) and 50% for 1-year-old human infants (1 out of 2 containers). Conditions marked with ∗ were significantly different from chance in one-sample t tests. Unmarked bars were not significantly different from chance (see Analysis Details). Current Biology 2006 16, 1736-1740DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Strategy Preferences: First Choice Percentage of subjects choosing to use either place or feature cues to retrieve the reward on trial 1 of the experiment (before receiving any feedback). Current Biology 2006 16, 1736-1740DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 4 Strategy preferences across the Hominidae Mean percent correct (± SE) for the place and feature conditions for all nonhuman great ape genera and human 1- and 3-year-old children. Means are adjusted for the respective chance levels of the different subject groups (see Figure 2). No statistics were computed for the separate nonhuman genera because of relatively small sample sizes in some of the groups. Qualitatively, Pongo, Gorilla, and Pan, as well as 1-year-old human infants, all display a similar pattern. In contrast 3-year-old infants show the opposite preference. Below is the phylogenetic tree displaying the evolutionary relationships among the four Hominid genera (Pongo, Gorilla, Pan, Homo). The photographs display individuals of four representative species. All five extant species of Hominids participated in the reported study: orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), gorilla (Gorilla gorilla), bonobo (Pan paniscus), chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), and human (Homo sapiens). Here, we assume a taxonomy based on monophyletic groups. In this framework, Hominidae include all the great apes including humans but not the Hylobatidae or small apes. Current Biology 2006 16, 1736-1740DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.049) Copyright © 2006 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions