Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon 

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Advertisements

From: Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions
The Evolution of Lateralization in Group Hunting Sailfish
Crossmodal Visual Input for Odor Tracking during Fly Flight
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Araceli Ramirez-Cardenas, Maria Moskaleva, Andreas Nieder 
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Musical Consonance: The Importance of Harmonicity
Responses to Spatial Contrast in the Mouse Suprachiasmatic Nuclei
Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Volume 23, Issue 18, Pages (September 2013)
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Volume 26, Issue 4, Pages (February 2016)
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Depth Affects Where We Look
Selective Attention in an Insect Visual Neuron
Volume 20, Issue 5, Pages (May 1998)
Volume 26, Issue 8, Pages (April 2016)
Volume 22, Issue 14, Pages (July 2012)
A Statistical Description of Plant Shoot Architecture
Learning Biases Underlie “Universals” in Avian Vocal Sequencing
Sensitivity to Temporal Reward Structure in Amygdala Neurons
Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness
Syed A. Chowdhury, Gregory C. DeAngelis  Neuron 
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Kensaku Nomoto, Susana Q. Lima  Current Biology 
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Visual Control of Altitude in Flying Drosophila
Gal Aharon, Meshi Sadot, Yossi Yovel  Current Biology 
Rapid Innate Defensive Responses of Mice to Looming Visual Stimuli
Efficacy of Thalamocortical and Intracortical Synaptic Connections
Adaptation Disrupts Motion Integration in the Primate Dorsal Stream
Attention Governs Action in the Primate Frontal Eye Field
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Jennifer L. Hoy, Iryna Yavorska, Michael Wehr, Cristopher M. Niell 
Optic Flow Cues Guide Flight in Birds
Walking Modulates Speed Sensitivity in Drosophila Motion Vision
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Mosquitoes Use Vision to Associate Odor Plumes with Thermal Targets
Volume 27, Issue 21, Pages e3 (November 2017)
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Restorative Justice in Children
Pattern and Component Motion Responses in Mouse Visual Cortical Areas
Mark S. Blumberg, Cassandra M. Coleman, Ashlynn I. Gerth, Bob McMurray 
Seasonal Encoding by the Circadian Pacemaker of the SCN
Caudate Microstimulation Increases Value of Specific Choices
Gilad A. Jacobson, Peter Rupprecht, Rainer W. Friedrich 
Visual Scene Perception in Navigating Wood Ants
Attention Reorients Periodically
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages (March 2008)
Volume 26, Issue 22, Pages (November 2016)
Raghav Rajan, Allison J. Doupe  Current Biology 
Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left
Tuning to Natural Stimulus Dynamics in Primary Auditory Cortex
Microtubule Severing at Crossover Sites by Katanin Generates Ordered Cortical Microtubule Arrays in Arabidopsis  Quan Zhang, Erica Fishel, Tyler Bertroche,
Visually Mediated Motor Planning in the Escape Response of Drosophila
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Ram Gal, Frederic Libersat  Current Biology 
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Goal-Driven Behavioral Adaptations in Gap-Climbing Drosophila
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
Søren K. Andersen, Steven A. Hillyard, Matthias M. Müller 
Gaby Maimon, Andrew D. Straw, Michael H. Dickinson  Current Biology 
Nonvisual Motor Training Influences Biological Motion Perception
A Temporal Channel for Information in Sparse Sensory Coding
Presentation transcript:

Cockroaches Keep Predators Guessing by Using Preferred Escape Trajectories  Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon  Current Biology  Volume 18, Issue 22, Pages 1792-1796 (November 2008) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.062 Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Individual Cockroaches Tested in Repeated Trials Show Similar, Striking Multimodal Distributions of ETs (A) Diagram of the sequence of movements in a typical cockroach escape response. The cockroach is walking from the left to the right of the picture. The gray arrow indicates stimulus direction. Wind angle (wind) at the time of stimulation, body turn (turn), and escape trajectory (ET) are shown. (B) Diagram illustrating the definition of escape trajectory (ET) and the way it is plotted in all subsequent figures. Escape trajectory (continuous arc) is defined as the angle between wind direction (gray arrow) and the direction of motion (black arrow) of the escaping cockroach (black cockroach). Left and right stimuli were pooled as if each stimulus was always on the right side of the animal. (C–G) The frequency distributions of five individual cockroaches tested in repeated trials. Numbers of responses are 93 (C), 93 (D), 89 (E), 81 (F), and 75 (G). The asterisk in (C) represents the trajectory of the escape response drawn in (B). Best-fit distributions in (C)–(G) are shown as multimodal curves. The statistics for the fitted curves are (C): Akaike weight = 0.91, χ2 = 14.77, p = 0.254, d.f. = 12; (D): Akaike weight = 0.93, χ2 = 19.87, p = 0.099, d.f. = 13; (E): Akaike weight = 0.85, χ2 = 8.98, p = 0.623, d.f. = 11; (F): Akaike weight = 0.51, χ2 = 10.48, p = 0.399, d.f. = 10; and (G): Akaike weight = 0.67, χ2 = 4.76, p = 0.854, d.f. = 9. For all panels, concentric circles represent a frequency interval of 2, bin intervals are 5°, and arrowheads indicate peaks, defined as those that contribute at least 5% to the best-fit curve. Current Biology 2008 18, 1792-1796DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.062) Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 The ET Distributions of Five Individuals Tested in Repeated Trials and 86 Singletons Tested Each Once Only Are Not Significantly Different from One Another (A) Frequency histograms of data set 5i (pooling five individuals from Figure 1). Total number of responses = 431, concentric circles represent a frequency of ten. Bin intervals are 5°. The best-fit curve Akaike weight > 0.99, χ2 = 23.71, p = 0.536, d.f. = 25. (B) Frequency histograms of singletons. Total number of responses = 86, concentric circles represent a frequency of two. Bin intervals are 5°. The best-fit curve Akaike weight = 0.74, χ2 = 5.48, p = 0.906, d.f. = 11. (C) The distribution of wind angles in escape responses (data set 5i) that showed little or no body turns (0°–10°; number of responses = 38). Concentric circles represent a frequency of two. Bin intervals are 5°. The wind angles show four peaks as in the preferred ETs found in (A) and (B); see text for details. The best-fit curve Akaike weight = 0.83, χ2 = 0.38, p = 0.943, d.f. = 3. For all panels, best-fit distributions are shown as multimodal curves and arrowheads indicate peaks, defined as those that contribute at least 5% to the best fit curve. (D) The effect of wind direction on ETs. Diagrammatic representation of the effect of wind angle on ET probability for the five individuals (data set 5i). Stimulus wind angle is indicated by the radial scale. The different ET peaks determined above for the 5i data are indicated by arrowheads at the appropriate angular positions, with the width of the arrowhead indicating the percentage contribution of that particular peak to the ET distribution at that particular wind angle. For scale, arrowheads are shown corresponding to 10%, 20%, and 50% contributions (inset). Peaks with a contribution of less than 5% are not shown. As wind angles increase, the lower value ET peaks (i.e., at approximately 90° and 120°) disappear and a 204° peak appears. Current Biology 2008 18, 1792-1796DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.062) Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Re-analysis of Cockroach Escape-Behavior Data from the Published Literature Reveals Hitherto-Undetected Patterns of ET Distributions (A) Concentric circles represent a frequency of two. Bin intervals are 5°. Number of responses = 161. Results are based on published data [6]. Akaike weight = 0.68, χ2 = 13.06, p = 0.907, d.f. = 21. (B) Concentric circles represent a frequency of 2.5. Bin intervals are 5°. Number of responses = 253. Results are based on published data [10]. Akaike weight = 0.76, χ2 = 27.19, p = 0.709, d.f. = 32. For both panels, the best-fit distribution is shown as a multimodal curve and arrowheads indicate peaks, defined as those that contribute at least 5% to the best-fit curve. Current Biology 2008 18, 1792-1796DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2008.09.062) Copyright © 2008 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions