Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Niche construction drives social dependence in hermit crabs
Advertisements

RNA-Directed DNA Methylation: Getting a Grip on Mechanism
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Erman Misirlisoy, Patrick Haggard  Current Biology 
Volume 18, Issue 15, Pages R651-R652 (August 2008)
Germán Sumbre, Graziano Fiorito, Tamar Flash, Binyamin Hochner 
Biomechanics: An Army Marching with Its Stomach
Invertebrate solutions for sensing gravity
Bacterial Size: Can’t Escape the Long Arm of the Law
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Volume 20, Issue 10, Pages R431-R435 (May 2010)
Musical Consonance: The Importance of Harmonicity
Pericycle Current Biology
Aaron R. Seitz, Praveen K. Pilly, Christopher C. Pack  Current Biology 
Generalizable Learning: Practice Makes Perfect — But at What?
Sizing up dogs Current Biology
Sensory-Motor Integration: More Variability Reduces Individuality
Visual Categorization: When Categories Fall to Pieces
Circatidal clocks Current Biology
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Linguistic Relativity: Does Language Help or Hinder Perception?
Cell Biology: Microtubule Collisions to the Rescue
Volume 22, Issue 13, Pages (July 2012)
Ian S. Howard, Daniel M. Wolpert, David W. Franklin  Current Biology 
Meiosis: Organizing Microtubule Organizers
Multisensory Integration: What You See Is Where You Hear
Kinesthetic information disambiguates visual motion signals
X-Inactivation: Xist RNA Uses Chromosome Contacts to Coat the X
Sleep: How Many Switches Does It Take To Turn Off the Lights?
Antarctic sea ice losses drive gains in benthic carbon drawdown
Visual Attention: Size Matters
Elephant cognition Current Biology
Volume 24, Issue 2, Pages R60-R61 (January 2014)
Volume 18, Issue 15, Pages R651-R652 (August 2008)
Volume 23, Issue 7, Pages R265-R266 (April 2013)
Lisa M. Fenk, Andreas Poehlmann, Andrew D. Straw  Current Biology 
Volume 22, Issue 20, Pages (October 2012)
Volume 26, Issue 16, Pages (August 2016)
What We Know Currently about Mirror Neurons
Aniruddh D. Patel, John R. Iversen, Micah R. Bregman, Irena Schulz 
Saving endangered whales at no cost
Sense of agency Current Biology
Locomotion: Why We Walk the Way We Walk
Volume 20, Issue 3, Pages (February 2010)
Volume 27, Issue 20, Pages R1106-R1107 (October 2017)
Restorative Justice in Children
Planar Cell Polarity: Microtubules Make the Connection with Cilia
Asif A. Ghazanfar, Daniel Y. Takahashi, Neil Mathur, W. Tecumseh Fitch 
It’s all about the constraints
Nonsomatotopic Organization of the Higher Motor Centers in Octopus
Pericycle Current Biology
Visual Development: Learning Not to See
Neural Coding: Bumps on the Move
Group Behaviour: Leadership by Those in Need
Insect Navigation: How Do Wasps Get Home?
Paolo Domenici, David Booth, Jonathan M. Blagburn, Jonathan P. Bacon 
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Knowledgeable Lemurs Become More Central in Social Networks
Decision-Making: Are Plants More Rational than Animals?
Volume 18, Issue 15, Pages R641-R645 (August 2008)
Invertebrate solutions for sensing gravity
Volume 19, Issue 9, Pages R353-R355 (May 2009)
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Sensory Neurophysiology: Motion Vision during Motor Action
Nadine Krüger, Iva M. Tolić-Nørrelykke  Current Biology 
Basal bodies Current Biology
Volume 18, Issue 5, Pages R198-R202 (March 2008)
The path of DNA in the kinetochore
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Presentation transcript:

Octopus Movement: Push Right, Go Left Scott L. Hooper  Current Biology  Volume 25, Issue 9, Pages R366-R368 (May 2015) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.066 Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Octopuses move by their arms pushing the body in the desired movement direction. (A) The proximal portion of arm 5 has shortened — the lines in each arm represent arm suckers; shortening shown by the lines in the proximal portion of arm 5 being closer together than in the other arms — and the suckers at the distal end of the shortened portion (black rectangle in arm) have attached to the substrate. Open arrows show direction of body movement that occurs when shortened region elongates. Distal portion of arm (red) does not move when proximal portion elongates (dashed and solid arrows in A and B show that this portion of the arm remains in a constant position relative to the environment, represented by the rectangular surrounds). (B) Octopus position after elongation of proximal portion of arm 5: animal has moved up and left. During elongation, velocities of the attached suckers and the distal portion of the arm are zero, and distances to the mouth for both increase in parallel as elongation proceeds (see Figure S2A in [1]). (C) Data in red show behaviorally observed percentage of movements in directions indicated by each point. Data in blue show percentage of movements in these directions predicted from frequencies with which different pairs of arms are co-activated. (Behavioral data from Figure 1C, arm activation data from Table S1, of [1]). Data are symmetrical around the 0°–180° axis because in [1] the behavioral data from positive and negative directions were combined by taking the absolute value of the angles (that is, all movements from –30° to +30° were reported in a 0° to +30° bin). I therefore also performed this treatment on the arm activation data. To produce the plot the percentages in each absolute value bin were divided by two and this value plotted at the appropriate positive and negative angle values (for example, the 15.9% of the behavioral data that occurred between angles of –30° to +30° were plotted as 7.95% at ±15°). Current Biology 2015 25, R366-R368DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2015.02.066) Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions