Training Attentional Control in Infancy

Slides:



Advertisements
Similar presentations
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Advertisements

Positive emotional contagion in a New Zealand parrot
Elizabeth V. Goldfarb, Marvin M. Chun, Elizabeth A. Phelps  Neuron 
David Pitcher, Bradley Duchaine, Vincent Walsh  Current Biology 
Volume 15, Issue 7, Pages (April 2005)
Pre-constancy Vision in Infants
Perirhinal-Hippocampal Connectivity during Reactivation Is a Marker for Object-Based Memory Consolidation  Kaia L. Vilberg, Lila Davachi  Neuron  Volume.
Theta-Coupled Periodic Replay in Working Memory
Volume 21, Issue 2, Pages (January 2011)
Vincent B. McGinty, Antonio Rangel, William T. Newsome  Neuron 
Differential Impact of Behavioral Relevance on Quantity Coding in Primate Frontal and Parietal Neurons  Pooja Viswanathan, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Sensorimotor Learning Configures the Human Mirror System
Volume 26, Issue 13, Pages (July 2016)
Volume 27, Issue 19, Pages e2 (October 2017)
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
The Generality of Parietal Involvement in Visual Attention
Cristina Márquez, Scott M. Rennie, Diana F. Costa, Marta A. Moita 
Cultural Confusions Show that Facial Expressions Are Not Universal
Disruption of Perceptual Learning by a Brief Practice Break
Damian M. Herz, Baltazar A. Zavala, Rafal Bogacz, Peter Brown 
Volume 74, Issue 3, Pages (May 2012)
Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala
Acetylcholine Mediates Behavioral and Neural Post-Error Control
The Occipital Place Area Is Causally Involved in Representing Environmental Boundaries during Navigation  Joshua B. Julian, Jack Ryan, Roy H. Hamilton,
Ryota Kanai, Tom Feilden, Colin Firth, Geraint Rees  Current Biology 
Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision
Near-Real-Time Feature-Selective Modulations in Human Cortex
Neuronal Response Gain Enhancement prior to Microsaccades
Serial, Covert Shifts of Attention during Visual Search Are Reflected by the Frontal Eye Fields and Correlated with Population Oscillations  Timothy J.
Dopaminergic Modulation of Arousal in Drosophila
Volume 25, Issue 5, Pages (March 2015)
Dynamics of Eye-Position Signals in the Dorsal Visual System
Direct Two-Dimensional Access to the Spatial Location of Covert Attention in Macaque Prefrontal Cortex  Elaine Astrand, Claire Wardak, Pierre Baraduc,
Social Signals in Primate Orbitofrontal Cortex
Peng Zhang, Min Bao, Miyoung Kwon, Sheng He, Stephen A. Engel 
Dissociable Effects of Salience on Attention and Goal-Directed Action
Masaya Hirashima, Daichi Nozaki  Current Biology 
Noa Raz, Ella Striem, Golan Pundak, Tanya Orlov, Ehud Zohary 
Volume 27, Issue 3, Pages (February 2017)
Attention Reorients Periodically
Motor Skills Are Strengthened through Reconsolidation
Masayuki Matsumoto, Masahiko Takada  Neuron 
Event Boundaries Trigger Rapid Memory Reinstatement of the Prior Events to Promote Their Representation in Long-Term Memory  Ignasi Sols, Sarah DuBrow,
Multisensory Integration and Attention in Developmental Dyslexia
Function and Structure of Human Left Fusiform Cortex Are Closely Associated with Perceptual Learning of Faces  Taiyong Bi, Juan Chen, Tiangang Zhou, Yong.
Federica Amici, Filippo Aureli, Josep Call  Current Biology 
Attention Samples Stimuli Rhythmically
Encoding of Stimulus Probability in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex
Volume 16, Issue 20, Pages (October 2006)
Ian C. Fiebelkorn, Yuri B. Saalmann, Sabine Kastner  Current Biology 
Volume 16, Issue 13, Pages (July 2006)
Claudia Lunghi, Uzay E. Emir, Maria Concetta Morrone, Holly Bridge 
Daniela Vallentin, Andreas Nieder  Current Biology 
Kevin R. Foster, Thomas Bell  Current Biology 
Sound Facilitates Visual Learning
Cross-Modal Associative Mnemonic Signals in Crow Endbrain Neurons
Social Information Signaling by Neurons in Primate Striatum
The challenge of measuring long-term positive aftereffects
Volume 21, Issue 7, Pages (April 2011)
Experience-Driven Plasticity in Binocular Vision
The Interaction between Binocular Rivalry and Negative Afterimages
Volume 20, Issue 8, Pages (April 2010)
Coupled Oscillator Dynamics of Vocal Turn-Taking in Monkeys
Impaired Associative Learning with Food Rewards in Obese Women
Memory Reactivation Enables Long-Term Prevention of Interference
Spatiotemporal Neural Pattern Similarity Supports Episodic Memory
Volume 23, Issue 11, Pages (June 2013)
Head-Eye Coordination at a Microscopic Scale
Volume 27, Issue 2, Pages (January 2017)
Volume 27, Issue 17, Pages e4 (September 2017)
Presentation transcript:

Training Attentional Control in Infancy Sam Wass, Kaska Porayska-Pomsta, Mark H. Johnson  Current Biology  Volume 21, Issue 18, Pages 1543-1547 (September 2011) DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.004 Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 1 Results from Training (A) Training: average difficulty level. The longest unbroken instance of each training task per session was identified, and the average difficulty level of each task was calculated (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Average difficulty level at visit 4 was normalized to 1, to allow comparison between the degrees of improvement at the different tasks. Error bars represent standard errors. (B) Training: training time. Gray lines show per-session training times for individual participants; the thick black line shows the average. The large change between visit 1 and visits 2–4 is because visit 1 was conducted immediately following the pretest assessment battery, so infants had already conducted circa 90 min of testing. Current Biology 2011 21, 1543-1547DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.004) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 2 Results of Pre-post Assessments All plots show Δ, the change in performance (post − pre, baseline corrected) in the trained and control groups. Error bars represent standard errors. Asterisks indicate significance of ANCOVA analyses as described in Results: ∗p < 0.05; ∗∗p < 0.01; (∗)p < 0.10. (A) Cognitive control. Graph shows proportion of correct anticipatory looks in the preswitch (initial rule learning) and postswitch (unlearning one rule and learning another) phases. (B) Gap overlap. “Average RT” is the average of the three conditions that we administered (gap, baseline, and overlap). “Facilitation” shows the facilitation effect, and “disengagement” shows disengagement latencies. Because the valence of the observed changes in the task was negative, −Δ values are presented for ease of comparison. (C) Sustained attention. “Mixed dynamic/static” shows the results of experiment 1, which measured looking behavior toward a mixture of dynamic and static stimuli. “Interesting static” and “boring static” show the results of experiment 2, which measured looking behavior toward “interesting” and “boring” static images. (D) Working memory. Graph shows median delay length for trials followed by a correct response. (E) Structured free play. “Looks to object” shows number of separate looks to the target objects. “Shifts from object to person” shows number of attention shifts from looking at the objects to looking at either the experimenter or caregiver. “Average duration of looks to object” shows the average duration of looks toward the target objects. Current Biology 2011 21, 1543-1547DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.004) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions

Figure 3 Results of Pre-post Assessments: Selected Scatter Plots from the Trained Group Showing Amount of Training Time against Change in Performance at Posttesting The y axes show the amount of total training time (in minutes) that each participant received. The x axes show trained infants' change in performance (post − pre) on three measures; in (B), −Δ values are presented for ease of comparison. For all three graphs, a position to the right of the y axis indicates improved performance posttraining. The regression lines indicate the significant bivariate correlations (see Results) observed between training time and outcome measures in (A) and (C). Current Biology 2011 21, 1543-1547DOI: (10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.004) Copyright © 2011 Elsevier Ltd Terms and Conditions